Skip to main content

The Generative Nature of Epistemological Judgments: Focusing on Interactions Instead of Elements to Understand the Relationship Between Epistemological Beliefs and Cognitive Flexibility

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Links Between Beliefs and Cognitive Flexibility

Abstract

The aim of this chapter is to discuss epistemological beliefs and epistemological judgments in relation to cognitive flexibility. Data from cognitive science and cognitive psychology are discussed to show that the human information processing system can be defined as a highly flexible system. From this perspective, cognitive flexibility can be seen to some degree as normality and not as an exceptional case (Statement 1: Cognitive flexibility is normality not an exceptional case). Against this background, the idea of a generative nature of epistemological judgments is presented. This perspective provides possible (re-)interpretations of conceptual and methodological problems of research on epistemological beliefs (Statement 2: Stability is normality in educational psychology and cognitive flexibility is the exceptional case). To examine the value of this idea, a research philosophy is necessary that focuses on interactions instead of separate cognitive elements to understand the flexibility of the cognitive system. Possible research designs are discussed how to use such an approach to get new insights into the claimed generative nature of epistemological judgments and the relationship between epistemological beliefs and cognitive flexibility (Statement 3: Focusing on detailed interactions between complementary cognitive elements as the smallest unit to understand the flexibility of epistemological judgments is necessary).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    “Context” is a complex notion. In this chapter, context is defined by the specific scientific content that a learner has to elaborate within a specific learning scenario like a school lesson, a seminar, or an informal learning setting. Thus, context is always seen in relation to a specific scientific content that has to be dealt with within a learning scenario.

  2. 2.

    The examples are not related to scientific knowledge, but they are appropriate to illustrate the critique.

  3. 3.

    When the instrument was developed, no differentiation between epistemological beliefs and epistemological judgments was made.

  4. 4.

    From this view, the distinction between cognition and metacognition is challenging. Further elaborations of the view might result in a denial of these constructs.

References

  • Anderson, L. W., Krathwohl, D. R., Airasian, P. W., Cruikshank, K. A., Mayer, R. E., Pintrich, P. R., et al. (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1987). The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101–140). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bromme, R. (2005). Thinking and knowing about knowledge: A plea for and critical remarks on psychological research programs on epistemological beliefs. In M. Hoffmann, J. Lenhard, & F. Seeger (Eds.), Activity and sign: Grounding mathematics education (pp. 191–201). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bromme, R., & Kienhues, D. (2008). Allgemeinbildung [general education]. In W. Schneider & M. Hasselhorn (Eds.), Handbuch der Pädagogischen Psychologie (pp. 619–628). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bromme, R., Kienhues, D., & Stahl, E. (2008). Knowledge and epistemological beliefs: An intimate but complicate relationship. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge and beliefs: Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 423–441). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bromme, R., Pieschl, S., & Stahl, E. (2010). Epistemological beliefs are standards for adaptive learning: A functional theory about epistemological beliefs and metacognition. Metacognition and Learning, 5, 7–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buehl, M. M., & Alexander, P. A. (2001). Beliefs about academic knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 385–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buehl, M. M., & Alexander, P. A. (2006). Examining the dual nature of epistemological beliefs. International Journal of Educational Research, 45, 28–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buehl, M. M., Alexander, P. A., & Murphy, P. K. (2002). Beliefs about schooled knowledge: Domain specific or domain general? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 415–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, G. A., & Grossberg, S. (2002). Adaptive resonance theory. In The handbook of brain theory and neural networks (2nd ed., pp. 87–90). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, K., & Elliot, R. G. (2002). Exploratory study of Hong Kong teacher education students’ epistemological beliefs: Cultural perspectives and implications on beliefs research. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 292–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. J., Hallett, D., & Sokol, B. W. (2002). Competing claims about competing know­ledge claims. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 145–168). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarebout, G., Elen, J., Luyten, L., & Bamps, H. (2001). Assessing epistemological beliefs: Schommer’s questionnaire revisited. Educational Research and Evaluation, 7, 53–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, T. M., & Knuth, R. A. (1990). Hypermedia and instruction: Where is the match? In D. H. Jonassen & H. Mandl (Eds.), Designing hypermedia for learning (pp. 199–225). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elby, A., Frederiksen, J., Schwarz, C., & White, B. (2003). Epistemological beliefs assessment for physical science (EBAPS). Retrieved from http//www2.physics.umd.edu/~elby/EBAPS/home.htm.

  • Elby, A., & Hammer, D. (2001). On the substance of sophisticated epistemology. Science Education, 85, 554–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elby, A., & Hammer, D. (2010). Epistemological resources and framing: A cognitive framework for helping teachers interpret and respond to their students’ epistemologies. In L. D. Bendixen & F. C. Feucht (Eds.), Personal epistemology in the classroom: Theory, research, and implications for practice (pp. 409–434). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Feltovich, P. J., Spiro, R. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1989). The nature understanding in biomedicine: The deep structure of complex development of misconceptions. In D. Evans & V. Patel (Eds.), Sciences in medicine (pp. 113–172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1987a). Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional form, color, and brightness perception: I. Monocular theory. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 87–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1987b). Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional form, color, and brightness perception: II. Binocular theory. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 117–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, D. (1994). Epistemological beliefs in introductory physics. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 151–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, D., & Elby, A. (2002). On the form of a personal epistemology. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 169–190). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, D., & Elby, A. (2003). Tapping students’ epistemological resources. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12, 53–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, B. K. (2000). Dimensionality and disciplinary differences in personal epistemology. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 378–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, B. K. (2006). Domain specificity of personal epistemology: Resolved questions, persistent issues, new models. International Journal of Educational Research, 45, 85–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, B. (2009, August). Online learning and epistemic understanding in adolescence: Preliminary findings. Paper presented at the 13th Biennial Conference, EARLI 2009, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J., & Spiro, R. J. (1995). Hypertext learning environments, cognitive flexibility, and the transfer of complex knowledge: An empirical investigation. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 12, 301–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jehng, J.-C. J., Johnson, S. D., & Anderson, R. C. (1993). Schooling and students’ epistemological beliefs about learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kienhues, D., Bromme, R., & Stahl, E. (2008). Changing epistemological beliefs: The unexpected impact of a short-term intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 545–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (1994). Developing reflective judgment: Understanding and ­promoting intellectual growth and critical thinking in adolescents and adults. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S. (1986). The reflective judgment model: Characteristics, evidence, and measurement. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.), Adult cognitive development (pp. 76–91). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Cheney, R., & Weinstock, M. (2000). The development of epistemological understanding. Cognitive Development, 15, 309–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Weinstock, M. (2002). What is epistemological thinking and why does it matter? In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 121–144). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Limon, M. (2006). The domain generality-specificity of epistemological beliefs: A theoretical, a methodological problem or both? International Journal of Educational Research, 45, 7–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louca, L., Elby, A., Hammer, D., & Kagey, T. (2004). Epistemological resources: Applying a new epistemological framework to science instruction. Educational Psychologist, 39, 57–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., & Boldrin, A. (2008). Epistemic metacognition in the context of information searching on the web. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge and belief: Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 377–404). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., & Boscolo, P. (2004). Role of epistemological understanding and interest in interpreting a controversy an in topic-specific belief change. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Future challenges and directions for theory and research on personal epistemology. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 389–414). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, S. A., Hammer, D., & Phelan, J. (2006). Multiple epistemological coherences in an eighth-grade discussion of the rock cycle. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15, 261–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & The PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1972). Dynamic memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and learning in computers and people. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherr, R. E., & Hammer, D. (2009). Student behavior and epistemological framing: Examples from collaborative active-learning activities in physics. Cognition and Instruction, 27, 147–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 498–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schommer, M., Calvert, C., Giana, G., & Bajaj, A. (1997). The development of epistemological beliefs among secondary students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 37–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., Bendixen, L. D., & Dunkle, M. E. (2002). Development and validation of the epistemic belief inventory (EBI). In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 261–275). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J., Feltovich, P. J., Jacobson, M. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1991). Cognitive flexibility, ­constructivism and hypertext: Random access instruction for advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. Educational Technology, 31, 24–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J., & Jehng, J. C. (1990). Cognitive flexibility and hypertext: Theory and technology for the nonlinear and multidimensional traversal of complex subject matter. In D. Nix & R. Spiro (Eds.), Cognition, education and multimedia: Exploring ideas in high technology (pp. 163–205). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, E. (2009, November). The generative nature of epistemological judgements. Keynote speech at the Personal Epistemology and Learning (PEL) Conference, Taipei, Taiwan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabak, I., & Weinstock, M. (2008). A sociocultural exploration of epistemological beliefs. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge and beliefs: Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 177–195). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 277–304). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elmar Stahl .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Stahl, E. (2011). The Generative Nature of Epistemological Judgments: Focusing on Interactions Instead of Elements to Understand the Relationship Between Epistemological Beliefs and Cognitive Flexibility. In: Elen, J., Stahl, E., Bromme, R., Clarebout, G. (eds) Links Between Beliefs and Cognitive Flexibility. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-1793-0_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics