Skip to main content

Case Matching and Conflicting Bindings Interference

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Case, Word Order and Prominence

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 40))

Abstract

Similarity-based interference (SBI) has recently gained more attention in the domain of sentence processing (e.g. AQ: Gordon et al. 2007 is cited in text but not given in the reference list. Please chec, Gordon et al. 2007). In this paper we demonstrate that similarity can also have facilitative effects on processing, a finding that interference theories such as Gordon et al’s cannot explain. We offer an explanation for such interference effects as well as the facilitative effects in terms of independently motivated assumptions about the structure of memory representations (Hommel, Vis Cogn 5:183–216, 1998; inter alia). An attractive aspect of this explanation of similarity-based interference and facilitation effects is that so-called case-matching phenomena can also be accounted for. To this end we present two experiments: In Experiment 1 we demonstrate that case matching can occur even with non-coreferent NPs, given a sufficient level of similarity. In Experiment 2 we show that case matching is really driven by abstract case proper as opposed to other properties canonically associated with it. In sum, we provide a unified explanation for interference, facilitation and case-matching effects. A broader implication of this account is that case ambiguities are not resolved immediately but rather the multiple representations are maintained in parallel – a mechanism that is clearly not compatible with serial parsing strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Retrieval interference occurs during a retrieval operation, e.g. while processing the head, but it is dependent on the relation between the target item and competitors.

  2. 2.

    There is one caveat to this experiment, however. In (2a) and (2d) the nouns involved match in gender, which is not so in (2b) and (2d). So the data do not exclude an interpretation in which gender and abstract case conspire to cause interference if both match, while the case marker does not have any effect at all.

  3. 3.

    We will furthermore assume throughout this paper, that B1 and B2 will conflict only if F1 and F2 are of the same type, e.g. case, gender, etc. This is only for expository reasons. Untyped bindings make the same predictions.

  4. 4.

    More formally, if two objects have n and all features belonging to an object are bound to each other every feature will be bound to \( (n-1)\) others. Since bindings are bidirectional, both objects will consist of \( ((n-1)+(n-2)+\dots +2+1)\) bindings each. If they match in k features, each object will contain \( k*(n-k)\)conflicts, because only bindings from matching to mismatching features are conflicting, and every object contains k of the former and \( (n-k)\) of the latter. If we increase similarity by making q more features match, the number of conflicts will be \((k+q)*(n-(k+q))\). An interference effect due to additional similarity corresponds to \( k*(n-k)<(k+q)*n-((k+q))\). One can easily show that this is the case iff k  <  n/2. Thus one can formally demonstrate that for low levels of similarity, CBRI predicts an interference effect if the objects are made more similar, while for high levels of similarity it predicts the opposite.

  5. 5.

    Admittedly, the exact differences between the numbers of matching and mismatching features for any two objects may appear to be a free parameter in the CBRI theory. They are, however, not completely unconstrained. Although precise quantitative predictions of CBRI rely on the objects’ similarity at least on an interval scale, we can still derive qualitative predictions from similarity rankings on an ordinal scale, available from uncontroversial intuitions and possibly similarity rating studies.

  6. 6.

    The self-paced reading method was used to ensure comparability of our results with the results obtained by Schlesewsky (1996) and Fanselow et al. (1999). The plausibility rating study on the other hand, served to exclude potential systematic differences in plausibility caused by the fact that the proposition of the sentence was not constant across conditions.

  7. 7.

    These questions were meant to direct the participants’ attention to the fact that the embedded clause object was a partitive construction and not a pronoun.

  8. 8.

    If the verb was preceded by an adverb (in two items), it was included in the region too. One item lacked an auxiliary, and so the region consisted of the verb only.

References

  • Anderson, J.R., and R. Paulson. 1978. Interference in memory for pictorial information. Cognitive Psychology 9: 178–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babyonyshev, M., and E. Gibson. 1999. The complexity of nested structures in Japanese. Language 75: 423–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M. 1996. Sprachverstehen. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M., and M. Meng. 1999. Case attraction phenomena in German. Unpublished Manuscript. University of Jena, Jena.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M., M. Meng, and J. Bayer. 2000. Case and reanalysis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 29: 37–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., and D. Sarkar. 2007. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes (R package version 0.9975-11) [Computer software].

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, J., M. Bader, and M. Meng. 2001. Morphological underspecification meets oblique case: Syntactic and processing effects in German. Lingua 111: 465–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, C., B. Juhasz, J. Ashby, M.J. Traxler, M.T. Mohamed, R.S. Williams, et al. 2003. The use of thematic role information in parsing: Syntactic processing autonomy revisited. Journal of Memory and Language 49: 317–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, C., A. Staub, and K. Rayner. 2007. Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In Eye movements: A window on mind and brain, ed. R. van Gompel, M. Fisher, W. Murray, and R.L. Hill, 341–372. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L.S., A. Raffone, and B. Hommel. 2006. What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 32: 705–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fanselow, G. 2000. Optimal exceptions. In Lexicon in focus, ed. B. Stiebels and D. Wunderlich, 173–209. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fanselow, G., M. Schlesewsky, D. Cavar, and R. Kliegl. 1999. Optimal parsing: Syntactic parsing preferences and optimality theory. Rutgers Optimality Archive, 367–1299. http://roa.rutgers.edu/.

  • Fedorenko, E., M. Babyonyshev, and E. Gibson. 2004. The nature of case interference in online sentence processing in Russian. NELS 34 conference proceedings. Amherst: GLSA Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., and K. Rayner. 1982. Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology 14: 178–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., and J. Hill. 2007. Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P.C., R. Hendrick, and M. Johnson. 2001. Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27: 1411–1423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P.C., R. Hendrick, and W.H. Levine. 2002. Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science 13: 425–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. 1998. Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition 5: 183–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., and L.S. Colzato. 2004. Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition 11: 483–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopf, J.-M., M. Bader, M. Meng, and J. Bayer. 2003. Is human sentence parsing serial or parallel? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research 15: 165–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A.W. 1984. Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23(5): 612–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, E. 1997. Processing subject-object ambiguities in Dutch. PhD thesis, University of Groningen

    Google Scholar 

  • König, E., and W. Lezius. 2003. The TIGER language – A description language for syntax graphs, formal definition (Technical Report). Germany: IMS, Universität Stuttgart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamers M.J.A. 2001. Sentence processing: using syntactic, semantic, and thematic information. PhD thesis. Groningen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamers, M.J.A. 2005. Resolving subject-object ambiguities with and without case: Evidence from ERPs. In Competition and Variation in Natural Languages: The Case for Case, ed. M. Amberber and H. de Hoop, 251–293. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R.L. 1996. Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25: 93–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R.L., and S. Vasishth. 2005. An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science 29: 1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R.L., S. Vasishth, and J. Van Dyke. 2006. Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(10): 447–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logačev, P., and S. Vasishth. (2006). The em package for computing eyetracking measures. Potsdam, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logačev, P. and S. Vasishth. (in preparation). Cue-less retrieval interference and facilitation. Ms, University of Potsdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mak, W.M., W. Vonk, and H. Schriefers. 2006. Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rock crush. Journal of Memory and Language 54(4): 466–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElree, B. 2000. Sentence comprehension is mediated by content-addressable memory structures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 29: 111–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, T. 2006. German inherent datives and argument structure. In Datives and similar cases, ed. A. Werner, H. Daniel, and M. André. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meseguer, E., M. Carreiras, and C. Clifton Jr. 2002. Overt reanalysis strategies and eye movements during the reading of mild garden path sentences. Memory & Cognition 30: 551–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., and N. Chomsky. 1963. Finitary models of language users. In Handbook of mathematical psychology, vol. II, ed. D.R. Luce, R.R. Bush, and E. Galanter. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., and R. Kliegl. 2006. A formal model of capacity limits in working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 55: 601–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Primus, B. 1999. Cases and thematic roles. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., and A. Pollatsek. 1987. Eye movements in reading: A tutorial review. In Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. M. Coltheart, 327–362. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheepers, C., B. Hemforth, and L. Konieczny. 2000. Linking syntactic functions with thematic roles: Psych-verbs and the resolution of subject-object. In German sentence processing, ed. B. Hemforth and L. Konieczny, 65–93. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlesewsky, M. 1996. Kasusphänomene in der Sprachverarbeitung. PhD thesis, Universität Potsdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlesewsky, M., G. Fanselow, R. Kliegl, and J. Krems. 2000. The subject preference in the ­processing of locally ambiguous wh-questions in German. In German sentence processing, ed. B. Hemforth and L. Konieczny, 65–93. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suckow, K., S. Vasishth, and R. Lewis. 2005. Interference and memory overload during parsing. Poster presented at the AMLaP conference, Ghent.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J.A., and R.L. Lewis. 2003. Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A retrieval interference theory of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language 49(3): 285–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J.A., and B. McElree. 2006. Retrieval Interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 55: 157–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S. 2008. Integration and prediction in head-final structures. In Processing and producing head-final structure. ed. H. Yamashita, Y. Hirose, and J. Packard. Springer, 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S., and R.L. Lewis. 2006. Argument-head distance and processing complexity: Explaining both locality and antilocality effects. Language 82(4): 767–794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S., S. Brüssow, R.L. Lewis, and H. Drenhaus. 2008. Processing polarity: How the ungrammatical intrudes on the grammatical. Cognitive Science 32(4): 685–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Gisbert Fanselow for very helpful suggestions concerning the design of Experiments 1 and 2, as well as Felix Engelmann, Esther Sommerfeld, and Titus von der Malsburg for extensive discussions of the ideas presented in this paper. Furthermore we thank Monique Lamers for very helpful comments on the first draft of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pavel Logačev .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Logačev, P., Vasishth, S. (2012). Case Matching and Conflicting Bindings Interference. In: Lamers, M., de Swart, P. (eds) Case, Word Order and Prominence. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 40. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-1463-2_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics