Skip to main content

The Interaction of Case, Word Order and Prominence: Language Production and Comprehension in a Cross-linguistic Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Case, Word Order and Prominence

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 40))

Abstract

We introduce the central topics of this volume, case, word order and prominence (i.e., an element’s ranking on a hierarchy of semantic features) and we illustrate their interaction through discussion of an example of a ‘who-did-what-to-whom’-ambiguity in Dutch. In the second part of the chapter we provide an overview of the different chapters included in the volume. From the different contributions it becomes clear that in each of the languages discussed in this volume case, word order and prominence are essential cues for speaker and hearer.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A note on case morphology in Dutch. Morphological case distinctions are absent on Dutch nouns (beside certain fixed expressions). In the pronominal system one generally finds a distinction between a subject form (nominative) and a non-subject form (sometimes called the objective and glossed here as such). The latter form is a syncretism of the accusative and dative case and covers the functions of both the direct object and indirect object. For some pronouns such as u (2sg polite) a further syncretism is found between the nominative and objective case form resulting in a fully ambiguous morphological form.

  2. 2.

    Another pressing issue is the interaction and relative importance of different prominence dimensions. Given that animates are more prominent than inanimates and that definites are more prominent than indefinites, how should indefinite animates be ranked with respect to definite inanimates? The interaction of prominence dimensions is taken up in the present volume in the chapters by Klein et al., Primus, Wang et al., Lamers and Kretzschmar et al. and is bound to remain a topic in future research.

  3. 3.

    Although not a type of prominence information, sometimes the subject function is (confusingly) said to be more prominent than the object function.

References

  • Aissen, J. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21: 435–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M., and M.J.A. Lamers. 2009. Case in comprehension. In Oxford handbook of case, ed. A. Malchukov and A. Spencer, 402–419. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M., J. Bayer, J-M. Hopf, and M. Meng. 1996. Case-Assignment in processing German verb-final clauses. Paper presented at NELS 26 Sentence Processing Workshop, MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blutner, R. 2000. Some aspects of optimality in natural language interpretation. Journal of Semantics 17: 189–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J.K., and R.K. Warren. 1985. Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation. Cognition 21: 47–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., and M. Schlesewsky. 2009. The role of prominence information in the real-time comprehension of transitive constructions: A cross-linguistic approach. Language and Linguistcs Compass 3: 19–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, H.P., M.J. Pickering, and M. Tanaka. 2008. Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production. Lingua 118: 172–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, J., A. Cueni, T. Nikitina, and R.H. Baayen. 2007. Predicting the dative alternation. In Cognitive foundations of interpretation, ed. G. Bouma, I. Krämer and J. Zwarts. Amsterdam: KNAW.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, Ö. 2008. Animacy and egophoricity: Grammar, ontology and phylogeny. Lingua 118: 141–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Hoop, H., and M.J.A. Lamers. 2006. Incremental distinguishability of subject and object. In Case, valency, and transitivity, ed. L. Kulikov, A. Malchukov, and P. de Swart, 269–287. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Swart, P. 2011. Sense and simplicity: Bidirectionality in differential case marking. In Bidirectional optimality theory, ed. A. Benz and J. Mattausch. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company 125–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Swart, P. 2007. Cross-linguistic variation in object marking. Utrecht: LOT Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dryer, M.S. 1992. The Greenbergian word order correlations. Language 68: 81–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J.H. 1966. Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Universals of Language, ed. J.H. Greenberg, 73–113. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gries, S.T.H. 2005. Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34: 365–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J.A. 1983. Word order universals. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., and M. Rappaport Hovav. 2005. Argument realization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Oshima, D. 2007. Syntactic direction and obviation as empathy based phenomena: A typological approach. Linguistics 45(4): 727–763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L., A. Kim, and G. Kuperberg. in press. The neurobiology of sentence comprehension. In The Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics, ed. M. Spivey, M. Joanaisse and K. McRae. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prat-Sala, M., and H.P. Branigan. 2000. Discourse constraints on syntactic processing in language production: A cross-linguistic study in English and Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language 42: 168–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siewierska, A. 1988. Word order rules. London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siewierska, A. 2004. Person. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlin, R.S. 1986. Basic word order: Functional principles. London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, M. 1999. Animacy and reference: A cognitive approach to corpus linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zúñiga, F. 2006. Deixis and alignment: Inverse systems in indigenous languages of the Americas. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Monique J. A. Lamers .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lamers, M.J.A., de Swart, P. (2012). The Interaction of Case, Word Order and Prominence: Language Production and Comprehension in a Cross-linguistic Perspective. In: Lamers, M., de Swart, P. (eds) Case, Word Order and Prominence. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 40. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-1463-2_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics