Skip to main content

Cultural and Blind-Specific Issues and Implications to Visual Thinking in Mathematics

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 1186 Accesses

Part of the book series: Mathematics Education Library ((MELI,volume 49))

Abstract

In this chapter, we explore cultural and blind-specific issues and their implications to visual thinking in mathematics. Issues on culture refer to those patterns of knowledge and skills, tool use, thinking, acting, and interacting that are favored by, and specific to, groups that support them. It is interesting to consider the possibility that students’ developing explicit knowledge in a lifeworld-dependent context interacts with macroconditions such as taken-as-shared cultural practices, which are likely to influence the way they perform visualization without them being aware of them.

Every culture, with its privileges or taboos of seeing, shapes a certain way of thinking, as it is in turn shaped by norms or vetoes of looking.

(Beltin, 2008 p. 189)

It is, of course, the norm rather than the exception that more than one sense modality contributes to the perception of an object. We hear, see, smell, and feel the same object. That is not because all senses provide the same information, but because the contributions from different modalities converge and overlap sufficiently to be felt as “same” rather than as “different.”

(Millar, 1994 p. 47)

Overall, neuropsychological reports, neuroimaging and behavioral findings support the view that visuo-spatial processes and numerical representation are intimately related. This relationship may constitute an early and fundamental link which, although partially shaped by our cultural constraints, remains an essential component of our cognitive architecture.

(de Hevia, Vallar, & Girelli, 2008 p. 1370).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For Baudrillard (1981/1972), the rationality of signs is seen in its ability to distinguish between signifier and signified and

    functions as the agent of abstraction and universal reduction of all potentialities and qualities of meanings that do not depend on or derive from the respective framing, equivalence, and specular relation of a signifier and signified. This is the directive and reductive rationalization transacted by the sign – not in relation to an exterior, immanent “concrete reality” that signs would supposedly recapture abstractly in order to express, but in relation to all that which overflows the schema of equivalence and signification; and which the sign reduces, represses, and annihilates in the very operation that constitutes it. … The rationality of the sign is rooted in its exclusion and annihilation of all symbolic ambivalence on behalf of a fixed and equational structure.

    (Baudrillard, 1981/1972, p. 149).

    I see social and cultural practices as providing conditions that engender the possibility of reduction, repression, and annihilation of sign ambivalence. The well-defined character of signs used in formal and institutional mathematical knowledge indicates this necessary elimination of ambivalence.

  2. 2.

    The term culture has many meanings and is certainly more complicated than the one that I describe in this chapter [cf. De Abreu, 2000; Lerman, 2001; Nunes 1992; Rivera & Becker, 2007b; special issue of the Journal of Intercultural Studies on culture in mathematics education edited by FitzSimons (2002)]. Byrne et al. (2004), for example, identify six dimensions of culture that merit individual analysis – that is, culture as pattern (social transmission processes); a sign of mind (solidified cognitive habits drawn from social transmission); a bonus (knowledge and practices resulting from social transmission); inefficiency (negative consequent actions resulting from social transmission); physical products (cultural actions and tools); and meaning (rituals). My characterization of culture is pragmatic; it has useful elements that allow me to explain the ideas I pursue later in relation to cultural forms of seeing in mathematics.

  3. 3.

    See footnote 4 in Chapter 2 (p. 55).

  4. 4.

    Zebian’s (2008) “ecologically-informed approach to numeric cognition” involves coordinating social and cognitive approaches. The cognitive approach “offers a way of thinking about how surface notation is recognizable and decoded and how these processes are related to semantically-based processes” (p. 362). The sociocultural approach “is acutely sensitive to the socially situated cognitive demands of numeric practices” (p. 362).

  5. 5.

    Were (2003) provides a thorough and engaging discussion of the object of kapkap, a patterned-shell ornament in the Western Island Melanesia, which he uses to demonstrate the rigor of mathematical thinking that comes with understanding its social and cultural significance. While the object conveys to these people certain “everyday and ritual performances,” it is the “translation from mental to material form that mobilizes mathematical thinking and spatial reasoning” (p. 42).

  6. 6.

    Raju (2001) also notes that Buddhists value inference as a valid means of validation but reject authoritative testimony, while Naiyayikas value all three (empirical, inference, authoritative testimony) means, including analogy (p. 328).

  7. 7.

    Ginsburg et al. (2003) also suggested the hypothesis that the teachers involved in the study influenced what the children were doing during free play. But this perspective was discounted due to the fact that the teachers observed were not seen interacting with the children during free play.

  8. 8.

    Morin became fully blind at age 6.

  9. 9.

    Females were excluded as the authors’ way of coping with less complicated data at the time of the study (i.e., holding the sex variable constant).

  10. 10.

    Ginns’s (2005) meta-analysis of 43 experimental studies prior to 2004 shows an overall strong modality effect (with moderation effects in some aspects), that is, “across a broad range of instructional materials, age groups, and outcomes, students who learned from instructional materials using graphics with spoken text outperformed those who learned from a graphics with printed text” (p. 326). A nonmathematical context that involves the use of nonvisual modality modes of learning among blind subjects involves studies in mobility (e.g., use of echoes, guide dogs, and long canes; sound cues; felt cues; electronic travel aids and ultrasonic echolocating prosthesis; cf. Strelow, 1985; Veraart & Wanet-Defalque, 1987).

  11. 11.

    Giroux was completely blind at age 11.

  12. 12.

    While recent haptic interface technologies in virtual reality provide externally induced compensatory strategies for blind learners due to loss in visual ability, they are also meant for them to develop useful and detailed spatial cognitive maps in long-term memory that will help improve their mobility and orientation skills (cf. Lahav, 2006).

References

  • Baudrillard, J. (1981/1972). For a critique of the political economy of the sign. St. Louis, MO: Telos Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belting, H. (2008). Perspective: Arab mathematics and renaissance Western art. European Review, 16(2), 183–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bier, C. (2000). Choices and constraints: Pattern formation in oriental carpets. Forma, 15, 127–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bier, C. (2006). Number, shape, and the nature of space: An inquiry into the meaning of geometry in Islamic art. In J. B. White (Ed.), How should we talk about religion? Perspectives, contexts, particularities (pp. 246–278). Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, A. (1979). Visualizing and mathematics in a pre-technological culture. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 10, 135–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewster, S. (2002). Visualization tools for blind people using multiple modalities. Disability and Rehabilitation, 24(11–12), 613–621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R., Barnard, P., Davidson, I., Janik, V., McGrew, W., Miklósi, A., et al. (2004). Understanding culture across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(8), 341–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Cornoldi, C., Mammarella, I., Bonino, D., Ricciardi, E., et al. (2008). Imagery and spatial processes in blindness and visual impairment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1346–1360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Abreu, G. (2000). Relationships between macro and micro sociocultural contexts: Implications for the study of interactions in the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 41, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Beni, R., & Cornoldi, C. (1988). Imagery limitations in totally congenitally blind subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(4), 650–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Hevia, M. D., Vallar, G., & Girelli, L. (2008). Visualizing numbers in the mind’s eye: The role of visuo-spatial processes in numerical abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1361–1372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. (1988). Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuropsychology. Psychological Review, 95(3), 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, M. (1997). When 1 + 1 ≠ 2: Making mathematics in central Brazil. American Ethnologist, 24(1), 132–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. (Ed.). (2002). Special issue: Cultural aspects of mathematics education. Journal of Intercultural Studies, 23(2), 109–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, J., Gardner, C., Jones, B., & Jones, E. (2008). Making arithmetic accessible to blind mainstream children. In Lecture notes in computer science (Vol. 5105): Proceedings of the 11th international conference on computers helping people with special needs (pp. 900–906). Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerdes, P. (2004). Mathematical thinking and geometric exploration in Africa and elsewhere. Diogenes, 202, 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15, 313–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H., Lin, C.-L., Ness, D., & Seo, K.-H. (2003). Young American and Chinese children’s everyday mathematical activity. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 5(4), 235–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heller, M., & Kennedy, J. (1990). Perspective taking, pictures, and the blind. Perception and Psychophysics, 48(5), 459–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, A. (2002). The world of blind mathematicians. Notices of the AMS, 49(10), 1246–1251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karshmer, A., & Farsi, D. (2008). Access to mathematics by blind students: A global problem. Journal of Systemics, Cybernetics, and Informatics, 5(6), 77–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. (1983). The role of vision in “visual imagery” experiments: Evidence from the congenitally blind. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112(2), 265–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N., Foulkes, D., & Schmidt, M. (1982). The structure of laboratory dream reports in blind and sighted subjects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170, 286–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahav, O. (2006). Using virtual environment to improve spatial perception by people who are blind. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 9(2), 174–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Gleitman, H., & Spelke, E. (1981). Spatial knowledge and geometric representation in a child blind from birth. Science, 213, 1275–1278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, S. (2001). Cultural, discursive psychology: A sociocultural approach to studying the teaching and learning of mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 46, 87–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Löwy, I. (2008). Ways of seeing: Ludwik Fleck and Polish debates on the perception of reality, 1890–1947. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 39, 375–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marmor, G., & Zabeck, L. (1976). Mental rotation by the blind: Does mental rotation depend on visual imagery? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2(4), 515–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millar, S. (1994). Understanding and representing space: Theory and evidence from studies with blind and sighted children. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I., Okamoto, Y., Vlahovic-Stetic, V., Kim, C., & Han, J. H. (1999). Language supports for childrens’ understanding of numerical fractions: Cross-national comparisons. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 356–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T. (1992). Ethnomathematics and everyday cognition. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 557–574). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan and National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özdural, A. (2000). Mathematics and arts: Connections between theory and practice in the medieval Islamic world. Historia Mathematica, 27, 171–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panofsky, E. (1991). Perspective as symbolic form (C. S. Wood, Trans.). New York: Zone Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2002). Signs and meanings in students’ emergent algebraic thinking: A semiotic analysis. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 42, 237–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raju, C. K. (2001). Computers, mathematics education, and the alternative epistemology of the calculus in the Yuktibhasa. Philosophy East & West, 51(3), 325–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieser, J., Guth, M., & Hill, E. (1986). Sensitivity to perspective structure while walking without vision. Perception, 15, 173–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rivera, F., & Becker, J. (2007b). Ethnomathematics in the global episteme: Quo vadis? In B. Atweh, A. C. Barton, M. Borba, N. Gough, C. Keitel, & R. Vithal (Eds.), Internationalization and globalization in mathematics and science education (pp. 209–226). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sawada, D. (1982). Multisensory information matching ability and mathematics learning. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 13(5), 390–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawada, D., & Juryman, R. F. (1978). Information matching within and between auditory and visual sense modalities and mathematics achievement. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 9(2), 126–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H., & Stigler, J. (1992). The learning gap: Why our schools are failing and what can we learn from Japanese and Chinese education. New York: Summit Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strelow, E. (1985). What is needed for a theory of mobility: Direct perception and cognitive maps – Lessons from the blind. Psychological Review, 92(2), 226–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinti, C., Galati, D., Vecchio, M. G., De Beni, R., & Cornoldi, C. (1999). Interactive auditory and visual images in persons who are totally blind. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 93(9), 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, D., & DeLoache, J. (1997). Manipulatives as symbols: A new perspective on the use of concrete objects to teach mathematics. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 37–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veraart, C., & Wanet-Defalque, M. C. (1987). Representation of locomotor space by the blind. Perception & Psychophysics, 42(2), 132–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J., & Meijer, P. (2010). Visual experiences in the blind induced by an auditory sensory substitution device. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 492–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wassmann, J. (1994). The Yupno as post-Newtonian scientists: The question of what is “natural” in spatial description. Man, 29(3), 645–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, H. (1990). Investigating the social foundations of mathematics: Natural number in culturally diverse forms of life. Social Studies of Science, 20, 283–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Were, G. (2003). Objects of learning: An anthropological approach to mathematics education. Journal of Material Culture, 8(1), 25–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, T., Williams, G., & McNeal, B. (2006). Children’s mathematical thinking in different classroom cultures. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37(3), 222–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zebian, S. (2008). Number conceptualization among Lebanese micro-business owners who engage in orally-based versus paper-based numeracy practices: An experimental cognitive ethnography. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 359–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, W., & Darron, C. (1999). Teaching statistics to a student who is blind. Teaching of Psychology, 26(2), 130–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ducker, L. (1993). Visually impaired students drawing graphs. Mathematics Teaching, 144, 23–26.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ferdinand D. Rivera .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rivera, F.D. (2011). Cultural and Blind-Specific Issues and Implications to Visual Thinking in Mathematics. In: Toward a Visually-Oriented School Mathematics Curriculum. Mathematics Education Library, vol 49. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-0014-7_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics