Skip to main content

Person Hierarchy Interactions in Syntax

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of Language

Part of the book series: Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory ((SNLT,volume 81))

  • 609 Accesses

Abstract

Chapter 3 uses the modular signature of syntax to show that some phi-agreement dependencies belong to syntax rather than realizational morphology. They are interactions between the transitive subject and object according to their person features, or person (hierarchy) interactions, in Algonquian, Mapudungun, and Arizona Tewa. The key finding is that the interactions are visible to other syntactico-semantic phenomena, unlike the morphological phenomena of Chapter 2. Other aspects of their ‘modular signature’ agree, notably operation across phrase-structurally unbounded domains. The person interaction of Arizona Tewa introduces the notion of a syntactic repair of a person (hierarchy) constraint for the subsequent chapters: the emergence of an otherwise unavailable syntactic structure in response to the impossibility of the regular one by a person constraint.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The term O is intended to mean the lower agreement controller in the agreement domain of EA, rather than its thematic co-argument, and so includes the embedded subject of cross-clausal agreement and raising-to-object (as in Algonquian below, or Picurís, Nichols 2001: 523, 531).

  2. 2.

    See Zuñiga (2002) for a lucid overview. Although it might appear that igw could contain i, this is not so on distributional and morphophonemic grounds: analyses such as Piggott (1989), Rhodes (1976) agree on this, although not on the gloss of i (see esp. Zuñiga 2002: 3.1–2).

  3. 3.

    The 3.PROX > 3.OBV hierarchy is actually 3 topic > 3 nontopic, which determines the former whenever one of the arguments is proximate because the proximate must be the unique topic, but also encompasses 3.OBV↔3.OBV that may be direct or inverse (Dahlstrom 1986: 53f., Klaiman 1992: 247; cf. Rhodes 1974: 211 note 5, Bruening 2001: 124).

  4. 4.

    Rhodes (1994: 443) notes that some speakers pick the EA as the pivot of cross-clausal obviation and/or cross-clausal agreement, rather than the winner of EA-O PH interaction (some picking EA for one diagnostic but the PH winner for the other). Dahlstrom (1986) finds that cross-clausal agreement in Plains Cree is not affected by the 3.PROX > 3.OBV hierarchy.

  5. 5.

    I do not add glosses to Rhodes's translations; the discussion below should clarify the examples.

  6. 6.

    The same Ban constrains 3↔3 contexts on the hierarchy 3.PROX→3.OBV. They are not illustrated here in detail, but they provide supporting evidence for the syntactic status of obviation. The tree (inanimate) hit John (animate) requires by the Ban that John be the winner on the hierarchy and thus proximate, giving an inverse configuration where O outranks EA. By contrast, John hit the tree is fine only if John is proximate, a direct configuration. These direct/inverse contrasts obtain reflected in the morphology in both the independent and conjuncts orders (Rhodes 1994: 433f.).

  7. 7.

    At issue is whether the pivot can reconstruct, which it should not be able to do via PRO unlike via A-movement (Burzio 1986). Bruening (2001: 131) does demonstrate for the Algonquian language Passamaquoddy that the inverse of 3EA→3O combinations not only allows O > EA scope, but also reconstructed EA > O scope, while the direct only allows EA > O.

  8. 8.

    Ini is optional when possible in these sentences, because all are also compatible with a matrix 3rd person animate non-topic pivot which would not trigger it, as well as a topic one. This is not relevant to the argument, which turns on the possibility of ini.

  9. 9.

    See further Bruening (2001), Mühlbauer (2008) on obviation. Superficially, the alternation in the obviation of the adjunct subject is reminiscent of that between anaphor types in sentences of the type Lynn i found Kate k tired when x i/k came home, in languages like Spanish, Finnish, or German. If x i picks up the subject, it tends to be pro rather than a pronoun, or a pronoun rather than a demonstrative, and inversely if x k picks up the object (Turan 1996, Arnold 1998, Kaiser 2003).

  10. 10.

    The 3.PROX > 3.OBV PH-interaction, which applies in the conjunct as well as independent order, matters both for determining α in the lower clause and for Bruening's generalization.

  11. 11.

    More precisely, the 1/2 > 3 portion of the hierarchy does. (12)b shows that in the 1EA→2O, O wins and creates an inverse, but there is no data on 2EA→1O (Bruening 2001: 277 note 10).

  12. 12.

    The 3.PROX-3.OBV distinction is inferred solely from the existence of two agreement patterns for 3↔3 interactions. The 1EA→2O combinations are special; Arnold calls them ‘middle'. Historically and in the Huichille dialect, there are transparent inverse forms, pe-e-ymi-Ø 'see-INV-2SG.SU –1.O' ‘I see you'. However, modern Mapudungun forms in the Tables ‘opaquely' express ‘I see you (SG)' by ‘We (two) see you', and ‘I/we see you' (total number of participants grater than two) by ‘We (plural) see each other' (see Chapter 2).

  13. 13.

    In 1↔2 scenarios, Arnold (1994: 38, 1997) observes that the EL-forms choose 2nd person in 1EA→2O and 2EA→1O, which Zuñiga (2002: 235) qualifies. Other syntactic correlates of the EA-O interaction in Mapudungun bear on or have only been tested for 3.PROX > 3.OBV. For instance, wh-movement applies to the O of morphologically direct and to the EA of morphologically inverse clauses, recalling the cross-linguistically common ban on ergative extraction and its repair in some Mayan languages by demotion to absolutive (Berinstein 1985, 1990, Davies and Sam Colop 1990, Hale 2001, Aissen 1999, Béjar and Rezac 2007: Appendix).

  14. 14.

    The inverse e is missing by assimilation to a (Arnold 1994: 40 note 10, 1997 note 14). The gloss of mün has been changed from you to your on the basis of Arnold's discussion.

  15. 15.

    I am grateful to A. Guillaume for discussion of Cavineña and providing me with his work.

  16. 16.

    For instance, in Movima, Haude (2006: 7.1–5), the winner of EA-O PH interaction must be overtly expressed and enclitic to the verb, with direct/inverse verbal markers indicating whether it is the EA or the O, according to the hierarchy 1SG > 1INCL/EXCL > 2SG > 2PL > 3 human > 3 non-human. This constraint ranges over both enclitics and full 3rd person DPs, making it appear syntactic rather than morphological. Moreover, the generalization has exceptions where the 2nd person in 1↔2 combinations is enclitic, but then the 1st person must still obey the constraint by being overt as a free pronoun, also suggesting a generalization going beyond clitics (Haude 2006: 278). In syntactic terms, the winner of the PH-interaction might move to a designated subjecthood position, which must be overt and control enclisis. Fascinating PH-interactions that fail to be clearly syntactic include Lakämper and Wunderlich (1998) for Quechua, and Dixon (2000), Farell (2005: 77ff.) for Jarawara; an example of a PH-interaction that has turned out to be spurious is Nichols' (1998: Chapter 2) reanalysis of Zuni from Nichols (1996), Albizu (1997b).

  17. 17.

    For other types of syntactic theories where phi-feature distinctions condition movement, see Laka (1993a), Johns (1993), Nash (1997), Hale (2001), Béjar (2003), Béjar and Rezac (2009), Baker (2008). Reference to phi-features may be under other terms. For Bruening (2001: 2.4.5), A-movement occurs through the feature [+P], but the 1st/2nd-3rd person distinction governs the distribution of [+P]: 1st/2nd person is inherently [+P], a 3rd person must not be [+P] if coargument of 1st/2nd person, while for two 3rd person coarguments one and one only is [+P].

  18. 18.

    The same issues appear to be reflected in the following claim for postsyntactic, realizational approaches to phi-agreement and case, which would be undermined by a PF principle like (15) to PH-interactions: “Syntactic ungrammaticality will not result from the realization of case and agreement. In particular, there is always a default case realization.” (Marantz 2000: 20)

  19. 19.

    3→3 combinations might reflect direct 3.PROXEA→3.OBVO vs. inverse 3.OBVEA→3.PROXO, as in Arnold (1994) for Mapudungun (cf. Kroskrity 1985: 315f.). Applicative constructions might shed more light on this. They treat the applicative object IO like the direct object of plain transitives (Allen and Frantz 1983: 308, Allen et al. 1990: 347). However, while 3EA→3O combinations can be direct or inverse and 1/2EA→XIO→3O are direct, 3EA→3IO→3O can only be inverse in Southern Tiwa (Rosen 1990: 2.3). This is consonant with person-like properties of even 3rd person applicative objects discussed in Section 5.2. (The IO prevents O from being 1st/2nd person in Southern Tiwa and Algonquian by the Person Case Constraint of Chapter 5, even when it is 3rd person and loses the PH-interaction to a 1st/2nd person EA; see Béjar and Rezac 2009: 46 note 6 for one approach, as well as Albizu 1997b, Lochbihler 2008, Boeckx 2008b.).

  20. 20.

    Kroskrity's agreement prefix glosses are modified in line with Zuniga 2002, as in Table 3.5.

  21. 21.

    See Medová (2009), Caha (2009) for work developing the idea that unmarked structural Case is due to movement from richer PP-like structures. The story can equally be explored in reverse according to another view of the ergative: the EA is base-generated bare, T attracts all EAs unless a 3rd person O intervenes, and it is movement to T that results in the assignment of an ergative oblique-like case (cf. Rezac 2003).

  22. 22.

    A PF mechanism that would then lend itself well to Arizona Tewa is lower-copy spell-out due to a PF constraint, along the lines developed by Bobaljik and Branigan (2006) for Chukchi (cf. Section 4.4). Both direct and inverse configurations would involve the same structure, (ia). The fully agreeing bare EA of direct combinations is the spell-out of the copy of the EA in [Spec, TP], (ib), forced when possible. A morphological constraint like (16) that the person of AGREA outrank AGRO prevents this in 3EA→1/2O and 1/2EA→2/1O combinations. It is resolved in (ib) by impoverishing AGREAwith consequent lower-copy spell-out of the EA in [Spec, vP], di-marked.

    (i)

    a [EA O V+T          [EA … O …]]

    Syntax

     

    b [EA O AGREA+AGRO+V+T [ …]]

    Spellout AGREA > AGRO

     

    c [ O AGREA+AGRO+V+T  [EA … …]]

    Spellout otherwise

    where top-copy EA spell-out is bare, lower-copy di-marked, and AGREA is impoverished if lower copy spell-out of EA occurs

  23. 23.

    For instance, both relativization and conjunction reduction might need PRO rather than a gap in Arizona Tewa, and there might not be an oblique PRO in the language.

  24. 24.

    Even so, the status of many hierarchies would remain unclear, for instance the rather natural classes defined by the interaction of person and number in Dummi (Trommer 2006).

References

  • Abney, Steven Paul. 1987. The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adger, David, and Daniel Harbour. 2007. Syntax and syncretisms of the Person Case Constraint. Syntax 10: 2–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aissen, Judith. 1997. On the syntax of obviation. Language 73: 705–750.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aissen, Judith. 1999. Agent focus and inversion in Tzotzil. Language 75: 451–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albizu, Pablo. 1997a. Generalized Person-Case Constraint: A case for a syntax-driven inflectional morphology. In Theoretical issues on the morphology-syntax interface, ed. Myriam Uribe-Etxebarria and Amaya Mendikoetxea, 1–33. Donostia: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albizu, Pablo. 1997b. The syntax of person agreement. Ms., University of Southern California, Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, Barbara J., and Donald G. Frantz. 1983. Advancements and verb agreement in Southern Tiwa. In Studies in Relational Grammar 1, ed. David M. Perlmutter, 303–316. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, Barbara J., and Donald Frantz 1986. Goal advancement in Southern Tiwa. IJAL 52: 388–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, Barbara J., Donald Franz, D. B. Gardiner, and David Perlmutter. 1990. Verb agreement multistratal representation in Southern Tiwa. In Studies in Relational Grammar 3, ed. Paul Postal and Brian Joseph, 321–383. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen. 1992. A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, Jennifer. 1994. Inverse voice marking in Mapudungun. In Proceedings of the BLS 20, 28–41. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, Jennifer. 1997. The inverse system in Mapudungun and other languages. Revista de Lingüística Teórica y Aplicada 34: 9–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, Jennifer. 1998. Reference forms and discourse patters. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford, CA: Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark. 2003. On the loci of agreement: inversion constructions in Mapudungun. In Proceedings of NELS 33, 25–49. Amherst, MA: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark. 2008. The syntax of agreement and concord. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Béjar, Susana. 2003. Phi-syntax: A theory of agreement. Doctoral dissertation, Toronto, ON: University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Béjar, Susana, and Milan Rezac. 2003. Person licensing and the derivation of PCC effects. In Romance linguistics: Theory and acquisition, ed. Anna-Teresa Pérez-Leroux and Yves Roberge, 49–62. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Béjar, Susana, and Milan Rezac. 2007. Cyclic Agree. Ms., University of Toronto, Toronto, Ont., and Université de Nantes. On-line: http://www.umr7023.cnrs.fr/sites/sfl/IMG/pdf/BR2005.pdf. Accessed September 4 2010.

  • Béjar, Susana, and Milan Rezac. 2009. Cyclic Agree. Linguistic Inquiry 40: 35–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berinstein, Ava. 1985. Evidence for multiattachment in K'ekchi Mayan. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berinstein, Ava. 1990. On distinguishing surface datives in K'ekchi. In Studies in Relational Grammar 3, ed. Paul M. Postal and Brian D. Joseph, 3–48. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi, Valentina. 1999. Consequences of antisymmetry: Headed relative clauses. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, Balthasar. 2007. Grammatical relations typology. Ms. Leipzig: University of Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobaljik, Jonathan David. 2008. Where's phi? In Phi theory, ed. Daniel Harbour, David Adger, and Susana Béjar, 295–328. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobaljik, Jonathan David, and Phillip Branigan. 2006. Eccentric agreement and multiple Case checking. In Ergativity: Emerging issues, ed. Alana Johns, Diane Massam., and Juvenal Ndayiragije, 47–77. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, Cedric. 2008b. The Person-Case Constraint and patterns of exclusivity. In Agreement restrictions, ed. Roberta D'Alessandro, Susann Fischer, and Gunnar Hrafn Hrafnbjargarson, 87–102. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, Phil, and Marguerite MacKenzie. 2001. Altruism, Ā-movement, and object agreement in Innu-aimûn. Linguistic Inquiry 2002: 385–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruening, Benjamin. 2001. Syntax at the edge: Cross-clausal phenomena and the syntax of Passamaquoddy. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burzio, Luigi. 1986. Italian syntax: A Government-Binding approach. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Caha, Pavel. 2009. The nanosyntax of Case. Doctoral dissertation, University of Tromsø / CASTL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlstrom, Amy. 1986. Plains Cree morphosyntax. Doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, William D., and Luis Enrique Sam-Colop. 1990. K'iche' and the structure of antipassive. Language 66: 522–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Déchaine, Rose-Marie. 1999. What Algonquian morphology is really like: Hockett revisited. In Papers from the Workshop on Structure and Constituency in Native American Languages, MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics 17, ed. Leora Bar-el, Rose-Marie Déchaine, and Charlotte Reinholtz, 25–72. Cambridge, MA: MIT, Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, MITWPL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delancey, Scott. 1981. An interpretation of split ergativity and related patterns. Language 57: 626–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diesing, Molly. 1992. Indefinites. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diesing, Molly, and Eloise Jelinek. 1995. Distributing arguments. Natural Language Semantics 3: 123–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. M. W. 1994. Ergativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. M. W. 2000. A-constructions and O-constructions in Jarawara. International Journal of American Linguistics 66: 22–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farell, Patrick. 2005. Grammatical relations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgi, Doreen. 2009. Local modelling of global Case splits. MA thesis, Leipzig: University of Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillaume, Antoine. 2006. Revisiting ‘split ergativity’ in Cavineña. International Journal of American Linguistics 72: 159–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, Kenneth. 2001. Eccentric agreement. In On Case and agreement, ed. Pablo Albizu and Beatriz Fernández, 15–48. Bilbao: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, Morris, and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The view from building 20, ed. Kenneth Hale and Jay Keyser, 111–176. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haude, Katharina. 2006. A grammar of Movima. Doctoral dissertation, Nijmegen: University of Nijmegen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heim, Irene. 1982. The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases. Doctoral dissertation, Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelinek, Eloise. 1993. Ergative ‘splits' and argument type. In MIT working papers in linguistics 18, 15–42. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelinek, Eloise, and Andrew Carnie. 2003. Argument hierarchies and the mapping principle. In Formal approaches to function in grammar, ed. Andrew Carnie, Heidi Harley, and MaryAnn Willie, 265–296. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelinek, Eloise, and Richard Demers. 1983. An agent hierarchy and voice in some Coast Salish languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 49: 167–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jelinek, Eloise, and Richard Demers. 1994. Predicates and pronominal arguments in Straits Salish. Language 70: 697–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johns, Alana. 1993. The occasional absence of anaphoric agreement in Labrador Inuttut. In Microparametrix syntax and dialect variation, ed. James R. Black and Virginia Montapanyane, 121–143. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, Elsi. 2003. The quest for a referent. Doctoral dissertation, Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klaiman, Mimi H. 1992. Inverse languages. Lingua 88: 227–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroskrity, Paul V. 1985. A holistic understanding of Arizona Tewa passives. Language 61: 306–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laenzlinger, Christopher. 1993. A syntactic view of Romance pronominal sequences. Probus 5: 241–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laka, Itziar. 1993a. The structure of inflection. In Generative studies in Basque linguistics, ed. José Ignacio Hualde and Jon Ortiz de Urbina, 21–70. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakämper, Renate and Dieter Wunderlich. 1998. Person marking in Quechua – A constraint-based minimalist analysis. Lingua 105: 113–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legate, Julie. 2008. Morphological and abstract case. Linguistic Inquiry 39: 55–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesourd, Philip. 1976. Verb agreement in Fox. In Harvard studies in syntax and semantics, vol. 2, ed. Jorge Hankamer and Judith Aissen, 445–528. Cambridge, MA: Department of Linguistics, Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lochbihler, Bethany. 2008. Person licensing: The Algonquian-Romance connection. In Proceedings of Canadian Linguistics Association 2008. On-line: //www.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2008/actes2008.html. Accessed September 4 2010.

  • Marantz, Alec. 2000 [1991]. Case and licensing. In Arguments and Case, ed. Eric Reuland, 11–30. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medová, Lucie. 2009. Reflexive clitics in the Slavic and Romance languages. Doctoral dissertation, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mühlbauer, Jeffrey. 2008. The representation of intentionality in Plains Cree. Doctoral dissertation, Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, Léa. 1997. La partition personnelle dans les langues ergatives. In Les pronoms: Morphologie, syntaxe et typologie, ed. Anne Zribi-Hertz, 129–150. Saint-Denis: Presses Universitaires de Vincennes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, Lynn. 1996. A constraint on A-positions and the Projection Principle. In Proceedings of ESCOL 95, 224–235. Ithaca: Cornell Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, Lynn. 1998. Topics in Zuni syntax. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, Lynn. 2001. The syntactic basis of referential hierarchy phenomena. Lingua 111: 515–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noyer, Rolf R. 1992. Features, positions, and affixes in autonomous morphological structure. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piggott, Glyne. 1989. Argument structure and the morphology of the Ojibwa verb. In Theoretical perspectives on Native American languages, ed. Donna B. Gerdts and Karin Michelson, 176–208. New York: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, Maria. 2003. Non-canonical agreement is canonical. Transactions of the Philological Society 101: 279–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rezac, Milan. 2003. The fine structure of cyclic Agree. Syntax 6: 156–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, Richard. 1976. The morphosyntax of the Central Ojibwa verb. Doctoral dissertation, Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, Richard. 1993. Syntax vs. morphology: A chicken and egg problem. Proceedings of BLS 19, Special session on syntactic issues in Native American languages, 139–147. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, Richard. 1994. Valency, inversion, and thematic alignment in Ojibwe. Proceedings of BLS 20, 431–446. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, Keren, and Leslie Saxon. 1994. The subject position in Athapaskan languages. In MIT working papers in linguistics 22, 173–195. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1997. The fine structure of the left periphery. In Elements of grammar, ed. Liliane Haegeman, 281–337. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, Carol. 1990. Rethinking Southern Tiwa: The geometry of a triple-agreement language. Language 66: 669–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, Michael. 1986. Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In Features and projections, ed. Peter Muysken and Henk van Riemsdijk, 163–232. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trommer, Jochen. 2001. Distributed Optimality. Doctoral dissertation, Potsdam, NY: University of Potsdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trommer, Jochen. Hierarchy-based competition and emergence of two-argument agreement in Dumi. Linguistics 44: 1011–1057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turan, Umit Deniz. 1996. Null vs. overt subjects in Turkish discourse. Doctoral dissertation, Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolford, Ellen. 1997. Four-way Case systems: Ergative, nominative, objective, and accusative. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 15: 181–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaenen, Annie, Joan Maling, and Höskuldur Thráinsson. 1985. Case and grammatical functions: The Icelandic passive. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3: 441–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zúñiga, Fernando. 2002. Inverse systems in indigenous languages of the Americas. Doctoral dissertation, Zürich: University of Zurich.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Milan Rezac .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rezac, M. (2010). Person Hierarchy Interactions in Syntax. In: Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of Language. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, vol 81. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9698-2_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics