Skip to main content

Conceptual Profile as a Model of a Complex World

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 968 Accesses

Part of the book series: Contemporary Trends and Issues in Science Education ((CTISE,volume 42))

Abstract

The conceptual profile theory is considered in this chapter from a sociocultural-historical perspective. From this point of view, this theory can be a candidate to represent the teaching-learning process in formal or nonformal school activities. Therefore, some aspects of this theory will be considered in order to point out the need for an approach relating the complexity of the world and language with the structural complexity of the subjects’ cognitive states. Our main working hypothesis is that the subjects’ cognitive structures are constituted with the intermediation of language. We consider that this complex structure is mediated and constituted by language and represent several different hierarchical levels of the potential complex relations among things in the world. We also stress the fact that conceptual profiles allow us to represent learning in context, dealing with the complexity of the cultural and historical dimensions of the representations used in our daily life. Finally, we understand that the structural complexity of the embodied and situated cognitive states could be expressed by a complex conceptual profile enabling us to represent not only its epistemological and ontological but also its axiological dimensions. In this work we intend to explore some potentialities of the conceptual profile theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Amit, D. J. (1989). Modeling brain function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bachelard, G. (1991). A filosofia do não. Lisboa: Abril Cultural.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays (C. Emerson & M. Holquist, Eds.) (V. W. McGee, Trans.). Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (1997). Estética da criação verbal [Aesthetics of verbal creation] (2ª ed.). São Paulo: Martins Fontes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (Volochinov, V. N. 2006). Marxismo e filosofía da linguagem: problemas fundamentais do método sociológico da linguagem [Marxism and philosophy of language] (M. Lahud y Y. F. Vieira, Trads.). São Paulo: Editora Hucitec.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastos, P. W., & Mattos, C. R. (2009). Um exemplo da dinâmica do perfil conceitual como complexificação do conhecimento cotidiano [An example of the conceptual profile dynamics as complexification of everyday knowledge]. Revista Electrónica de Enseñanza de las Ciencias, 8, 1054–1078.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (2000). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, B. (1990). The structuring of pedagogic discourse (Class, codes and control, Vol. IV). London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bondía, J. L. (2002). Notas sobre a experiência e o saber de experiência [Notes on experience and the knowledge of experience]. Revista Brasileira de Educação, 19, 20–28. doi:10.1590/S1413-24782002000100003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brophy, J. (1999). Toward a model of value aspects of motivation in education: Developing appreciation for particular learning domains and activities. Educational Psychologist, 34, 75–85. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3402_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castellani, B., & Hafferty, F. W. (2009). Sociology and complexity science: A new field of inquiry (Understanding complex systems series, Vol. XV). Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Charlot, B. (2001). A noção de relação com o saber: bases de apoio teórico e fundamentos antropológicos [The notion of relation with knowledge: Theoretical and anthropological foundations]. In B. Charlot (Ed.), Os jovens e o saber: perspectivas mundiais (pp. 15–31). Porto Alegre: Artmed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain body and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1995). Socio-cultural-historical psychology: Some general remarks and a proposal for a new kind of cultural-genetic methodology. In J. V. Wertsch, P. del Rio, & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 187–214). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (2003). Vygotsky and context: Where did the connection come from and what difference does it make? In N. Stevenson, H. Lorraine Radtke, R. Jorna, & H. Stam (Eds.), Theoretical psychology: Critical contributions (pp. 20–29). Toronto: Captus Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalri, J. (2009). A dimensão axiológica do perfil conceitual [An axiological dimension of the conceptual profile]. Master’s thesis, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eiser, J. R. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in social cognition. The Behavior and Brain Sciences, 42(21), 638–638. doi:10.1017/S0140525X98331738.43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J., et al. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y., & Miettinen, R. (1999). Introduction. In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen, & R.-L. Punamäki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: An activity-theoretical approach to developmental research. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiori, E. M. (2011). Aprender a dizer a sua palavra [Learning to say your word]. In P. Freire (Ed.), Pedagogia do oprimido (50th ed.). Rio de Janeiro: Editora Paz e Terra.

    Google Scholar 

  • French, R. M., & Thomas, E. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis: One battle behind (commentary). The Behavior and Brain Sciences, 21, 640–641. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X98361737

  • French, R. M., & Thomas, E. (2001). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science: A review essay of mind as motion. Minds and Machines, 11, 101–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2008). The phenomenological mind: An introduction to philosophy of mind and cognitive science. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1985). A nova ciência da mente: Uma história da revolução cognitiva [A new mind’s science: A history of cognitive revolution]. São Paulo: Edusp, 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geirsson, H., & Losonsky, M. (Eds.). (1996). Readings in language and mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimes, D., & Mozer, M. C. (2001). The interplay of symbolic and subsymbolic processes in anagram problem solving. In T. K. Leen, T. Dietterich, & V. Tresp (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (Vol. 13, pp. 17–23). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, U., & Chater, N. (1997). Concepts and similarity. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 43–92). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heit, E. (1997). Knowledge and concept learning. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 7–41). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertz, J. A., Krogh, A., & Palmer, R. G. (1991). Introduction to the theory of neural computation. Redwood City, CA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. (1995). Hidden order: How adaptation builds complexity. Cambridge, MA: Perseus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontiev, A. (1978). O desenvolvimento do psiquismo (M. D. Duarte, Trad.). Lisboa: Livros Horizonte.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lomônaco, J. F. B., et al. (2004). Concepções de cegueira de estudantes de um curso de psicologia [Conceptions about blindness among students of a psychology course]. Boletim de Psicologia, São Paulo, LIV(120), 23–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lomônaco, J. F. B., et al. (2001). Desenvolvimento de conceitos: o paradigma das transformações. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 17(2), 161–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ilyenkov, E. (1982). The dialectics of the abstract and the concrete in Marx’s Capital. Moscow: Progress.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, T. D. (2003). Symbolic and sub-symbolic representations in computational models of human cognition: What can be learned from biology? Theory & Psychology, 13, 847–860. doi:10.1177/0959354303136005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S. (2003). Cognitive coordination dynamics. In W. Tschacher & J. P. Dauwalder (Eds.), The dynamical system approach to cognition: Concepts and empirical paradigms based on self-organization, embodiment, and coordination dynamics (pp. 45–70). London: World Scientific.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Knowlton, B. (1997). Declarative and nondeclarative knowledge: Insights from cognitive neuroscience. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts and categories (pp. 215–246). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamberts, K., & Shanks, D. (1997). Knowledge, concepts and categories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurence, S., & Margolis, E. (1999). Concepts and cognitive science. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), Concepts: Core readings (pp. 3–81). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mainzer, K. (2007). Thinking in complexity: The computational dynamics of matter, mind and mankind. Heidelberg/Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos, C. R. (2010). O ABC da Ciência [The ABC of science]. In N. Garcia, I. Higa, E. Zimmermann, C. Silva, & A. Martins (Eds.), A pesquisa em ensino de Física e a sala de aula: articulações necessárias (Vol. 1, pp. 141–156). São Paulo: SBF.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos, C. R., & Rodrigues, A. M. (2007a). Theoretical considerations about conceptual profile dynamic. In European Science Education Research Association (ESERA-2007), Malmö. Proceedings ESERA 2007 (pp. 1–10). Malmö: University Malmö.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos, C. R., & Rodrigues, A. M. (2007b) Context and discursive activity in physics classroom. In VIII European Physics Education Conference (GIREP), Opatija. Proceedings (pp. 1–9). Rijeka: Faculty of Arts and Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos, C. R., & Viggiano, E. (2007). An instrument for assessment of the conceptual profiles of to teach and to learn. In VIII European Physics Education Conference (GIREP), Opatija. Proceedings (pp. 1–6). Rijeka: Faculty of Arts and Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos, C. R., & Viggiano, E. (2009) A methodology to assess conceptual profiles within context delimitation. In European Science Education Research Association (ESERA-2009). Proceedings of ESERA 2009 (pp. 1–10). Istanbul: ESERA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos, C. R., Dalri, J., & Rodrigues, A. M. (2008). Internalization and activity: Learning as complex system dynamic. In International Society for Cultural and Activity Research Congress ISCAR, 2008, San Diego. Book of Abstracts (Vol. 1., p. 126). San Diego, CA: ISCAR/UCSD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, R. H., & Varela, F. (1995). A árvore do conhecimento: as bases biológicas do entendimento humano [The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding]. São Paulo: Psy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F. (1998). Can connectionism save constructivism? Cognition, 66, 153–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Smith, E. E. (1981). Strategies and classification learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 241–253. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.7.4.241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meshcheryakov, B. G. (2007). Terminology in L. S. Vygotsky’s writings. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge Companion to Vygotsky (pp. 155–177). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mortimer, E. F. (1995). Conceptual change or conceptual profile change? Science & Education, 4, 265–287. doi:10.1007/BF00486624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mortimer, E. F. (2000). Linguagem e formação de conceitos no ensino de ciências. [Language and concept formation in science education]. Belo Horizonte: Editora UFMG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (Ed.). (1979). Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gerzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211–227. doi:10.1002/sce.3730660207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raczaszek-Leonardi, J., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2008). Reconciling symbolic and dynamic aspects of language: Toward a dynamic psycholinguistics. New Ideas in Psychology, 26, 193–207. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodrigues, A. M., & Mattos, C.R. (2006) A noção de contexto no ensino de ciências [The notion of context in the teaching of science]. In XXII Encuentros de Didáctica de las Ciencias Experimentales, 2006, Zaragoza. Actas del XXII Encuentros de Didáctica de las Ciencias Experimentales (pp. 01–08). Zaragoza: Universidad de Zaragoza.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodrigues, A. M., & Mattos, C. R. (2010). Towards understanding conceptual formation in science education. Cultural-Historical Psychology, 4, 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodrigues, A. M., & Mattos, C. R. (2011). Contexto, negociación y actividad en una clase de física [Context, negotiation and activity in physics classroom]. Enseñaza de Las Ciencias, 29, 201–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1979). On the architecture of intersubjectivity. In R. Rommetveit & R. M. Blakar (Eds.), Studies of language, thought, and verbal communication (pp. 93–108). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, F. (1958). The perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain. Psychological Review, 65, 386–408. doi:10.1037/h0042519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, F. (1962). Principles of neurodynamics. New York, NY: Spartan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense making in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334–337). New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sepulveda, C. (2010). Perfil conceitual de adaptação: Uma ferramenta para a análise de discurso em salas de aula de biologia em contextos de ensino de evolução [Conceptual profile of adaptation: A tool for analysing biology classroom discourse in the context of teaching evolution]. Doctoral dissertation, Federal University of Bahia and State University of Feira de Santana, Graduate Studies Program in History, Philosophy, and Science Teaching, Salvador.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. (1997). Distributed representations and implicit knowledge: A brief introduction. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts and categories (pp. 197–214). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sodré, F. C. R. (2008). Física para uma alimentação saudável [Physics for a healthy nutrition]. Master’s thesis, University of São Paulo, São Paulo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. (1999). The race for consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Gelder, T. (1995). What might cognition be, if not computation? The Journal of Philosophy, 92, 345–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gelder, T. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. The Behavior and Brain Sciences, 21, 615–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Oers, B. (1998). The fallacy of decontextualization. Mind, Culture and Activity, 5, 135–142. doi:10.1207/s15327884mca0502_7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotski, L. S. (1934/2001). A construção do pensamento e da linguagem [The construction of thought and language] (P. Bezerra, Trans.). São Paulo: Martins Fontes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank my research group for helping me to think about the world, my world, our world, and how to seek out ways to overcome our existential solipsism. Thanks to André Machado Rodrigues, Esdras Viggiano da Silva, Jaqueline Dalri, Luciani Tavares, Fernanda Cavaliere Sodré, Patrícia Weishpaut Bastos, José Luis Ortega, Francisco Amancio Mendes, Roberto Nicioli Junior, Felipe Prado, and Juliano Camillo. I also thank CNPq, CAPES, and Fapesp for the financial support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cristiano Rodrigues de Mattos .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

de Mattos, C.R. (2014). Conceptual Profile as a Model of a Complex World. In: Mortimer, E., El-Hani, C. (eds) Conceptual Profiles. Contemporary Trends and Issues in Science Education, vol 42. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9246-5_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics