Skip to main content

Filler-Gap Processing in Mandarin Relative Clauses: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Processing and Producing Head-final Structures

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 38))

Abstract

This chapter reports the results of an Event-Related Potential (ERP) study of relative clause processing in Mandarin Chinese. The objective of this research is to determine whether electrophysiological evidence of filler-gap integration costs found for other languages would be observed in Mandarin and to determine where in the relative clause filler-gap integration occurs. To pursue these goals, we analyze ERP data obtained from 20 Mandarin speakers as they read sentences containing subject-gap and object-gap relative clauses modifying the subjects and objects of matrix sentences. Our results indicate a larger P600 ERP component for subject-gap than object-gap relative clauses at the point in the clause where the filler is integrated with the antecedent gap. That effect was found on the relative clause marker de for clauses modifying the matrix subject and on the relative clause head for clauses modifying the matrix object. We interpret our results as supporting the hypothesis that the difficulty of integrating verb arguments is reflected in the magnitude of the P600 ERP component and that in Mandarin relative clauses, subject-gap arguments are more difficult to integrate than object-gap arguments. Based on these findings, we argue that in Mandarin, relative clause verb selectional restrictions may be satisfied in real time either by the relative clause head or by the relative clause marker de, depending on where the relative clause is located within the matrix sentence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The context affecting an incoming word’s interpretation can include other knowledge such as discourse pragmatic information (e.g., Tanenhaus, Carlson & Trueswell, 1989) in addition to the lexico-semantic and syntactic contextual information that we will focus on here.

  2. 2.

    Several proposals have been offered to account for processing difficulty in object-gap relatives, including accessibility of the relativized NP (Keenan & Comrie, 1977), differences in perspective between the relative clause and matrix sentence (MacWhinney, 1982; MacWhinney & Pleh, 1988), canonical vs. non-canonical word order in the relative clause (MacDonald & Christiansen, 2002), preference for the role of agent to be expressed by the clause-initial NP (Diessel & Thomasello, 2005) and distance between filler and gap, either in linear terms, measured by the number of words or constituents that intervene between the filler and the gap (King & Just, 1991; Kluender & Kutas, 1993; Gibson, 1998; Hsiao & Gibson, 2003), or in structural terms, measured by differences between subject-gap and object-gap relatives in the depth of embedding of the filler versus the gap (Lin and Bever, 2006; O’Grady, Lee, & Choo, 2003).

  3. 3.

    King and Kutas did directly compare the ERP profiles of the relative clause verbs in subject-gap and object-gap clauses but the area they examined was the 200–500 ms interval post-verb-onset, which is earlier than would have been necessary to observe a P600 effect.

  4. 4.

    See also the corpus study by Wu, Kaiser and Andersen, this volume, who found subject-gap relatives to be more frequent, thus potentially easier to process, than object-gap relatives.

References

  • Altmann, G., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boland, J., Tanenhaus, M., Garnsey, S., & Carlson, G. (1995). Verb argument structure in parsing and interpretation: Evidence from wh-questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 774–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diessel, H., & Thomasello, M. (2005). A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses. Language, 81.4, 882–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felser, H., Clahsen, H., & Münte T. (2003). Storage and integration in the processing of filler-gap dependencies: An ERP study of topicalization and wh-movement in German. Brain and Language, 87, 345–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiebach, C., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. (2002). Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: the processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 250–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A., Hahne, A., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). The temporal structure of syntactic parsing: early vs. late effects elicited by syntactic anomalies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1219–1248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A., Mecklinger, A., Spencer, K. M., Steinhauer, K., & Donchin, E. (2001). Syntactic parsing preferences and their on-line revisions: A spatio-temporal analysis of event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 305–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frisch, S., & Schlesewsky, M. (2001). The N400 reflects problems of thematic hierarchizing. Neuroreport, 12, 3391–3394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frisch, S., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). The resolution of case conflicts from a neurophysiological perspective. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 484–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frisch, S., Schlesewsky, M., Saddy, D., & Alpermann, A. (2002). The P600 as an indicator of syntactic ambiguity. Cognition, 85, B83–B92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 69, 1–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E., Desmet, T., Watson, D., Grodner, D., & Ko, K. (2005). Reading relative clauses in English. Cognitive Linguistics, 16–2, 313–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, J. (1979). Complement selection and the lexicon. Linguistic Inquiry, 10(2), 279–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsiao, F. (2003). The syntax and processing of relative clauses in Mandarin Chinese. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Ph.D. dissertation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsiao, F., & Gibson, E. (2003). Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition, 90, 3–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 98, 122–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaan E., Harris A., Gibson E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 159–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamide, Y., Altmann, G., & Haywood, S. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E. L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 63–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, J., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, J., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word and clause-related ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 378–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 196–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hilyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, T. (1983). Handedness questionnaire (Chinese version). Acta Psychologica, 15, 268–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, X, Shu, H., Liu, Y., & Li, P. (2006). Mental representation of verb meaning: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1774–1787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, C.-J., & Bever, T. (2006). Chinese is no exception: Universal subject preference of relative clause processing. Paper presented at the 19th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, New York, NY, March 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y., & Garnsey, S. (2006). Relative clause comprehension in Mandarin. Poster presented at the 46th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomics Society, Toronto, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y., & Garnsey, S. (2007). Plausibility and the resolution of temporary ambiguity in relative clause comprehension in Mandarin. Poster presented at the 20th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, University of California, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y., & Garnsey, S. (this volume). Animacy and the resolution of temporary ambiguity in relative clause comprehension in Mandarin. In H. Yamashita, Y. Hirose & J. Packard (Eds.), Processing and producing head-final structures. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M., & Christiansen, M. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1999). Psychological Review, 109, 35–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language acquisition, vol. 1: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., & Pleh, C. (1988). The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition, 29, 95–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, J., Hirotani, M., & Friederici A. (2007). ERP evidence for different strategies in the processing of case markers in native speakers and non-native learners. BMC Neuroscience, 8, 18. Published online 2007 March 2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, H., King, J., & Kutas, M. (1997). Event-related potentials elicited by spoken relative clauses. Cognitive Brain Research, 5, 193–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H., Nicol, J., Barss, A., Forster, K., & Garrett, M. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady, W., Lee, M., & Choo, M. (2003). A subject-object asymmetry in the acquisition of relative clauses in Korean as a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 433–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. (1993). Event-related potentials and syntactic anomaly: Evidence of anomaly detection during the perception of continuous speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 413–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P., & Swinney, D. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 20(4), 786–803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, D. (1993). Topic…comment. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 11, 557–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Abada, S. (2005). ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, A. (1974). On the role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clauses in English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 272–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, A., & Wu, Z. (1999). The syntax and interpretation of sentence-final DE. Proceedings of NACCL, 10, 257–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M., Carlson, G., & Trueswell, J. (1989). The role of thematic structures in interpretation and parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 211–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Traxler, M., Morris, R., & Seely, R. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 69–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Traxler, M., Williams, R., Blozis, S., & Morris, R. (2005). Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(2), 204–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan & G. Miller (Eds.), Lingusistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, F., Kaiser, E., & Andersen, E. (this volume). Subject preference, head animacy, and lexical cues: A corpus study of relative clauses in Chinese. In H. Yamashita, Y. Hirose & J. Packard (Eds.), Processing and producing head-final structures. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Xiaoming Jiang and Longjun Yu for help in running subjects, Gary Huang for help in Presentation programming, Hengqing Chu for help in locating subjects and Susan Garnsey, Gary Dell, Kara Federmeier, Kay Bock and Kiel Christiansen for helpful feedback and advice. We especially thank James Yoon for his input on c-selectional and s-selectional restrictions. Our appreciation also goes to the Research Board at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign for funding this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jerome L. Packard .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Packard, J.L., Ye, Z., Zhou, X. (2010). Filler-Gap Processing in Mandarin Relative Clauses: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. In: Yamashita, H., Hirose, Y., Packard, J. (eds) Processing and Producing Head-final Structures. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 38. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9213-7_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics