Skip to main content

Adolescent Narrative Thought: Developmental and Neurological Evidence in Support of a Central Social Structure

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover The Developmental Relations among Mind, Brain and Education

Abstract

In this chapter we discuss how young adolescents gradually develop the capacity to interpret human intentions and, in so doing, develop a deeper understanding of the social world, as evidenced in their stories and interpretations of events in their lives. Robbie Case’s theory is utilized to explain how and why developmental change occurs in the domain of narrative thought. More specifically, we argue that growth in working memory capacity permits the assembly of increasingly complex narrative structures and that working memory capacity sets an upward limit on development. Further, we link such cognitive growth to neurological research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The developmental scoring schemes are available from the first author on request.

References

  • Applebee, A. (1978). The child’s concept of story. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children’s metarepresentational understanding. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 85–103). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, P. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further exploration of children’s narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beach, R., Appleman, D., Hynds, S., & Wilhelm, J. (2006). Teaching Literature to Adolescents. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beach, R., & Wendler, L. (1987). Developmental differences in response to a story. Research in the Teaching of English, 21(3), 286–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettelheim, B. (1976). The uses of enchantment: The meaning and importance of fairy tales. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, J. M., Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Caggiano, D. M., Bennett, S. M., & Hommer, D. W. (2004). Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: Similarities and differences from young adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 1793–1802.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blishen, B. R., Carroll, W. K., & Moore, K. (1987). The 1981 socioeconomic index for occupations in Canada. Canadian Review of Sociology and Anthropology, 24, 465–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. T., Lugar, H. M., Coalson, R. S., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., Schlaggar, B. L. (2005). Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 275–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruchkowsky, M. (1992). The development of empathic understanding in early and middle childhood. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 153–170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, S. A., & Wright, S. B. (2007). Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 243–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. (1988). The power of myth. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1993). Theories of learning and theories of development. Educational Psychologist, 28(3), 219–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & Griffin, S. (1990). Child cognitive development: The role of central conceptual structures in the development of scientific and social thought. Developmental Psychology: Cognitive, perceptuo-motor, and neuropsychological Perspectives (pp. 193–230). North Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Griffin, S., McKeough, A., & Okamoto, Y. (1992). Parallels in the development of children’s social numerical, and spatial thought. In R. Case (Ed.), The Mind’s Staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 269–284). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & McKeough, A. (1990). Schooling and the development of central conceptual knowledge structures: An example from the domain of children’s narrative. International Journal of Education, 13(8), 835–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Marini, Z., McKeough, A., Dennis, S., & Goldberg, J. (1986). Horizontal structure in middle childhood: The emergence of dimensional operations. In S. Strauss and I. Levine (Eds.), State and structure in children’s development (pp. 1–39). New York: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 104–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ciesielski, K. T., Lesnik, P. G., Savoy, R. L., Grant, E. P., & Ahlfors, S. P. (2006). Developmental neural networks in children performing a Categorical n-back task. Neuroimage, 33, 980–990.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colom, R., Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2006). Distributed brain sites for the g-factor of intelligence. Neuroimage, 31, 1359–1365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Culler, J. (1975). Structuralists Poetics: Structuralism, linguistics, and the study of literature. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget, M., Bonnetblanc, F., & Duffau, H. (2007). Contrasting acute and slow-growing lesions: a new door to brain plasticity. Brain, 130, 898–914.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (Eds.) (2001). Beginning literacy with language. Baltimore, MD: Brooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K. (1989). Teaching as storytelling: An alternative approach to teaching and curriculum in the elementary school. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elston, G. N. (2003). Cortex, cognition and the cell: New insights into the pyramidal neuron and prefrontal function. Cerebral Cortex, 13(11), 1124–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, C., Bruner, J., Kalmar, D., & Renderer, B. (1993). Plot, plight, and dramatism: Interpretation at three ages. Human Development, 36(6), 327–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, C., Bruner, J., Kalmar, D., & Renderer, B. (1994). Plot, plight, and dramatism: Interpretation at three ages. In W. F. Overton & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), The nature and ontogenesis of meaning (pp. 225–277). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Haden, C. A. (2003). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Hudson, J. (Eds.). (1992). Knowing and remembering in young children. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frangou, S., Chitins, X., & Williams, S. C. R. (2004). Mapping IQ and gray matter density in healthy young people. Neuroimage, 3, 800–805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. P. (1991). Memory and myth: A perspective on narrative. In A. McCabe, & C. Peterson, (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 1–26). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Genereux, R., & McKeough, A. (2007). Transformation in narrative thought during adolescence: The structure and content of narrative interpretation. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77 (3), 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg-Reitman, J. (1992). Young girl’s conception of their mother’s role: A neo-structural analysis. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 135–152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grady, C. L., & Keightley, M. L. (2002). Studies of altered social cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders using functional imaging. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 47, 327–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, S., & Case, R. (1995). Rethinking the primary school math curriculum: an approach based on cognitive science. Issues in Education, 3, 1–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, S., Case, R., & Siegler, R. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 25–49). Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford Books,

    Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (5), 748–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C. (2003). Joint encoding and joint reminiscing: Implications for young children’s understanding and remembering of personal experiences. In R. Fivish & C. A. Haden, (Eds.). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 49–69). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, S. B. (1986). Taking a cross-cultural look at narrative. Topics in Language Disorders, 7, 84–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, D. (1994). Individual and social meanings in the classroom: Narrative discourse as a boundary phenomenon. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 4(3), 215–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. A., Gebelt, J., Haviland, J., & Bentivegna, C. (1992). Emotion and narrative structure in young children’s personal accounts. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 2(2), 129–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutsler, J. J. (2003). The specialized structure of human language cortex: Pyramidal cell size asymmetries within auditory and language-associated regions of the temporal lobes. Brain and Language, 86, 226–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvey, M., McKeough, A., & Pyryt, M. (2008). Teaching trickster tales. Research in the Teaching of English, 42(1), 42–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jernigan, T. L., & Tallal, P. (1990). Late childhood changes in brain morphology observable with MRI. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 379–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, A. E., Baldo, B. A., Pratt, W. E., & Will, M. J. (2005). Corticostriatal-hypothalamic circuitry and food motivation: Integration of energy, action and reward. Physiology and Behavior, 86, 773–795.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T. (2006). Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2171–2177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucian, K., von Aster, M., Loenneker, T., Dietrich, T., Mast, F. W., & Martin, E. (2007). Brain activation during mental rotation in school children and adults. Journal of Neural Transmission, 114, 675–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leichman, M. D., Wang, Q, & Pillemer. D. B. (2003). Cultural variations in interdependence and autobiographical memory: Lessons from Korea, China, India, and the United States. In R. Fivish, & C. A. Haden, (Eds.). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 73–97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mall, V., Linder, M., Herpers, M., Schelle, A., Mendez-Mendez, J., Korinthenberg, R., et al. (2005). Recruitment of the sensorimotor cortex – A developmental fMRI study. Neuropediatrics, 36, 373–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of story schema. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1414–1434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marini, Z., & Case, R. (1994). Development of abstract reasoning about the physical and social world. Child Development, 65(1), 147–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1993). The stories we live by: Personal myths and the making of the self. New York: William and Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, A. (1996). Chameleon readers: Teaching children to appreciate all kinds of good stories. New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, A., Capron, E., & Peterson, C. (1991). The voice of experience: The recall of early childhood and adolescent memories by young adults. In A. McCabe and C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 137–173). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (Eds.). (1991). Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConaughy, S. H., Fitzhenry-Coor, I., & Howell, D. C. (1984). Developmental differences in schemata for story comprehension. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol. 4, pp. 385–421). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A. (1996). Changes in narrative knowledge across the school years: Evidence from typical and atypical populations. In A. Smorti (Ed.). The self as text: Construction of stories and construction of the self (pp. 275–307). Florence, IT: Giunti.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A. (2000). Building on the oral tradition: How story composition and comprehension develop. In J. Astington (Ed.). Minds in the making (pp. 98–114). Cambridge, England: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A. (1995). Teaching narrative knowledge for transfer in the early school years. In A. McKeough, J. Lupart, & A. Marini (Eds.), Teaching for transfer: Fostering generalization in learning (pp. 153–176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough A. (1992a). The structural foundations of children’s narrative and its development. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 171–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough A. (1992b). Testing for the presence of a central social structure: Use of the transfer paradigm. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 207–225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A. (1991). Neo-Piagetian theory goes to school: Program development and program delivery. Exceptionality Education Canada, 1(4), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A., Davis, L., Forgeron, N., Marini, A., & Fung, T. (2005). Improving story complexity and cohesion: A developmental approach to teaching story composition. Narrative Inquiry, 15(2), 96–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A., & Genereux, R. (2003). Transformation in narrative thought during adolescence: The structure and content of story compositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 537–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A., & Sanderson, A. (1996). Teaching storytelling: A microgenetic analysis of developing narrative competency. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 6(2), 157–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A., Yates, T., & Marini, A. (1994). Intentional reasoning: A developmental study of behaviorally aggressive and normal boys. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 285–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, S. (1981). Sharing time: Children’s narrative style and differential access to literacy. Language in Society, 10, 423–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, S. (1991). The dismantling of narrative. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 303–351). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex dysfunction. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of the self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17 (2), 292–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. J., Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., & Liang, C. (1997). Personal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American families. Child Development, 68 (3), 557–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mosenthal, W. (1995). A textbook of neuroanatomy. London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U., & Fivush, R. (Eds.). (1994). The remembering self: Construction and accuracy in the self narrative. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2003). Narrative and self, myth and memory: Emergence of the cultural self. In R. Fivish, & C. A. Haden, (Eds.). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 1–28). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2000) The developmental psychology of language and thought. In M. Bennett (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Achievements and prospects (pp. 185–204). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1981). Social cognition in a script framework. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. 97–118). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolopoulou, A. (1997). Children and narratives: Toward an interpretive and sociocultural approach. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 179–216). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nippold, M. A., & Taylor, C. L. (1995). Idiom understanding in youth – Further examination of familiarity and transparency. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 426–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (1990). Thinking about narrative. In B. K. Britton & A. D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language (pp. 99–111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palombo, J. (1991). Neurocognitive differences, self cohesion, and incoherent self narratives. Child and Adolescent Social Work, 8(6), 449–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the human brain: A review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Research Bulletin, 54, 255–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelletier, J., & Astington, J. W. (2004). Action, consciousness and theory of mind: Children’s ability to coordinate story characters’ actions and thoughts. Early Education and Development, 15 (1), 5–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pfefferbaum, A., Mathalon, D. H., Sullivan, E. V., Rawles, J. M., Zipursky, R. B., & Lim, K. O. (1994). A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Archives of Neurology, 51, 874–887.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polkinghorne, D. E. (1988). Narrative knowing and the human sciences. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porath, M. (1992). Stage and structure in the development of children with various types of “Giftedness.” In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 303–317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2004). Hebb’s neural networks support the integration of psychological science. Canadian Psychology, 45, 265–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, M. W., Boyes, C., Robins, S., & Manchester, J. (1989). Telling tales: Working memory and the narrative cohesion of story retellings. Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 628–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Propp, V. (1968). The morphology of the folktale. Austin: University of Texas Press (Translated by L. Scott, 2nd edition. Revised by L. A. Wagner. Austin: University of Texas Press.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Quartz, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 537–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivkin, M. J. (2000). Developmental neuroimaging of children using magnetic resonance techniques. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research, 6, 68–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romine, C. B., & Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Woolley, J., Nosarti, C., Heyman, I., Taylor, E., et al. (2006). Progressive increase of frontostriatal brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event-related tasks of cognitive control. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 973–993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runyan, W. M. (1982). Life histories and psychobiographies. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakai, K. L. (2005). Language acquisition and brain development. Science, 310, 815–819.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarbin, (1986). Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human conduct. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology, 7, 273–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaefer, R. (2004). Narrating, Attending, and Empathizing. Literature and Medicine, 23(2), 241–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaefer, R. (1981). Narration in psychoanalytic dialogue. In W. J. T. Mitchell (Ed.), On narrative (pp. 25–49). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherf, K. S., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of developmental change in visuospatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18 1045–1058.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholes, R., & Kellogg, R. (1966). The nature of narrative. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., et al. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 676–679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Mapping changes in the human cortex throughout the span of life. The Neuroscientist, 10, 372–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 8223–8231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Rex, D., Kornsand, D., Tessner, K. D., Jernigan, T. L. , et al. (2002). Mapping sulcal pattern asymmetry and local cortical surface gray matter distribution in vivo: Maturation in perisylvian cortices. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 17–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sowell, E. R., Mattson, S. N., Thompson, P. M., Jernigan, T. L., Riley, E. P., & Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping callosal morphology and cognitive correlates – Effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurology, 57, 235–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, D. P. (1982). Narrative truth and historical truth: Meaning and Interpretation in psychoanalysis. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, M. D., Moorhead, T. W. J., Lymer, G. K. S., Job, D. E., Muir, W. J., Hoare, P., et al. (2006). Structural correlates of intellectual impairment and autistic features in adolescents. Neuroimage, 33, 1136–1144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steen, R. G., Ogg, R. J., Reddick, W. B., & Kingsley, P. B. (1997). Age related changes in the pediatric brain: Quantitative MR evidence of maturational changes during adolescence. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 18, 819–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of children’s storytelling skill. In M. B. Franklin & S. S. Barten (Eds.), Child language: A reader (282–299). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, K. E., Fink, G. R., & Marshall, J. C. (2007). Mechanisms of hemispheric specialization: Insights from analyses of connectivity. Neuropsychologia, 45 209–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sun, L. (1998). The loss of narrative innocence: The development of narrative consciousness from child to adolescent. Narrative Inquiry, 8 ( 1), 203–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton-Smith, B. (1986). Children’s fiction making. In T. R. Sarbin (Ed.), Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human conduct (pp. 67–90). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tappan, M., & Packer, M. (Eds.). (1991). Narrative and storytelling: Implications for understanding moral development. New Directions for Child Development, 54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoma, P., & Daum, I. (2006). Neurocognitive mechanisms of figurative language processing – Evidence from clinical dysfunctions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 1182–1205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 148–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Hoekstra, R. A., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Endophenotypes for intelligence in children and adolescents. Intelligence, 35, 369–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volpe, J. J. (1995). Neurology of the newborn. Philadelphia: WB Saunders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yussen, S. R., & Ozcan, N. M. (1996). The development of knowledge about narratives. Issues in Education, 2, 1–68.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne McKeough .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

McKeough, A., Griffiths, S. (2010). Adolescent Narrative Thought: Developmental and Neurological Evidence in Support of a Central Social Structure. In: Ferrari, M., Vuletic, L. (eds) The Developmental Relations among Mind, Brain and Education. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3666-7_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics