Skip to main content

Interviewing: An Insider’s Insight into Learning

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 922 Accesses

Abstract

Interviewing is often thought of as a research tool, but by shifting focus, teachers can use “interviewing” to support two important classroom goals: clarifying student understanding, and in turn providing students with opportunities to organize more meaningful structures of understanding. We provide an operational definition for levels of understanding and a hierarchical framework (similar to Robbie Case’s developmental framework) for evaluating changes in student understanding during the interview process. From a very different perspective, the student’s insights are viewed from the brain’s central organizational principle; brains learn about patterns in the world in order to make reliable predictions about its environment. Thus, new insights are new patterns that students recognize and can test against reality within carefully constructed classroom activities or during the interview process. Pattern recognition and changes in neural activity offer an additional dimension for understanding the dynamic interaction between the teacher/interviewer and the student, which we also explore in detail.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Even though this experience allowed Eve to encounter an interesting relationship, Mr. M decided to use C size batteries instead of D batteries in later lessons, which generates much less heat.

References

  • Ausubel, D. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, V. (1999). Students’ reasoning about chemical reactions: What changes occur during a context-based post-16 chemistry course. International Journal of Science Education, 21(6), 645–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bredderman, T. (1983). Effects of activity-based elementary science on student outcomes. Review of Educational Research 53(4), 499–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (Ed.). (1991). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of cognitive development. Special Issue: The role of frontal lobe maturation in cognitive and social development. Brain & Cognition, 20(1), 51–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, R., Guesne E., & Tiberghien, A. (1985). Some features of children’s ideas and their implications for teaching. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, & A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s ideas in science (pp. 193–201). Milton Keynes, UK: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, R., Squires, A., Rushworth, P., & Wood-Robinson, V. (1994). Making sense of secondary science: Research into children’s ideas. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T.R. (2006). Dynamic development of action and thought. Theoretical Models of Human Development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 313–339). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., Bullock D., Rotenberg, E. J., & Raya, P. (1993). The dynamics of competence: How context contributes directly to skill. In R. Wozniak & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments (pp. 93–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Pipp, S. L. (1984). Process of cognitive development: Optimal level and skill acquisition. In R. J. Sternber (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 45–80). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Rose S. P. (1998). Growth cycles of brain and mind. Educational Leadership, 56(3), 56–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J., & Blakeslee, S. (2004). On intelligence. New York: Times Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holt, J. (1964). How children fail. New York: Dell Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hugh, H., & Novak, J. D. (1983). <proc>Proceedings of the international seminar on misconceptions in science and mathematics</proc>. Ithaca, NY: Cornel University ED, 242, 553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightman, A., & Sadler, P. (1993). Teacher predictions versus actual student gains. The Physics Teacher, 31, 162–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. D. (1992). The public understanding of science and technology in the United States, 1990. Washington, DC: Report to the National Science Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V. B. (1998). Perceptual neuroscience: The cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nardi, B. (1996). Studying context: A comparison of activity theory, situated action models, and distributed cognition. In B. Nardi(Ed.), Context and consciousness: Activity theory and human-computer interaction (pp. 69–102). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novak, J. D., & Gowin D. B. (1984). Learning how to learn. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1993). Practices of distributed intelligence and designs for education. In G. Salomon(Ed.), Distributed cognitions: Psychological and educational considerations (pp. 47–87). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, A. (1988). Winnicott. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadler, P. (1998). Psychometric models of student conceptions in science: Reconciling qualitative studies and distractor-driven assessment instruments. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35(3), 265–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24(2), 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneps, M. H., & Sadler P. M. (1988). Private universe. Santa Monica, CA: Pyramid Films.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. S. (2000). Design challenges: A new path to understanding science concepts and skills. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. S., & Fischer K. W. (2004). Building general knowledge and skill: Cognition and microdevelopment in science learning. In A. Demetriou & A. Raftopoulos(Eds.), Emergence and transformation in the mind: Modeling and measuring cognitive change. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. S., & Sadler, P. S. (2007). Empowerment in science curriculum development: A microdevelopmental approach. International Journal of Science Education, 29(8), 987–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shamos, M. H. (1995). The myth of scientific literacy. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipstone, D. (1985). Electricity in simple circuits. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, & A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s ideas in science (pp. 33–51). Milton Keynes, UK: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thatcher, R. C. (1994). Cyclic Cortical Re-Organization: Origins of Human Cognitive Development. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theory. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 563–634). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity and chaos. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., Bruner J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marc S. Schwartz .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schwartz, M.S., Fischer, K.W. (2010). Interviewing: An Insider’s Insight into Learning. In: Ferrari, M., Vuletic, L. (eds) The Developmental Relations among Mind, Brain and Education. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3666-7_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics