Skip to main content

Conceptual Change: Creativity, Cognition, and Culture

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Origins: Studies in the Sources of Scientific Creativity ((ORIN,volume 3))

Abstract

Concept formation and change—what I here call “conceptual innovation”—is one of the most creative dimensions of scientific practice. Throughout the history of the sciences changes in representational structure have provided “revolutionary” understandings of nature. As with other creative outcomes, conceptual revolutions are still widely perceived to be the outcomes of mysterious acts of individual genius, such as represented by an Isaac Newton, a Charles Darwin, or an Albert Einstein. The object of this paper is to dispel this notion by establishing how to incorporate both the undoubtedly unique contributions of individual scientists and the inherently socio-cultural nature of all scientific creations into the analysis of conceptual innovation. The route to meeting this objective lies in interpreting the conceptual practices scientists employ as deriving both from aspects of mundane human cognitive capabilities and from the social and cultural contexts, scientific and ordinary, in which they are embedded. What is required to construct such an interpretation is 1) knowledge of pertinent aspects of human cognition, 2) knowledge of specific practices implicated in cases of conceptual innovation, and 3) an understanding of how social and cultural contexts provide conceptual, analytical, and material resources that shape such practices.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22:577–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobrow, D. G. (1985). Qualitative Reasoning About Physical Systems. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boden, M. A. (1990). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., Collins, A., and Duguid, S. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18:32–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, G. N. (1985). Reading the book of nature: The relation between Faraday's religion and his science. In Gooding and James, 1985, pages 69–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, N. (1989). Nature's Capacities and Their Measurement. Clarendon, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., and Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5:121–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (1989). Learning via model construction and criticism. In Glover, G., Ronning, R., and Reynolds, C., editors, Handbook of Creativity: Assessment, Theory, and Research, pages 341–381. Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. (1943). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darden, L. (1991). Theory Change in Science: Strategies from Mendelian Genetics. Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Kleer, J. and Brown, J. S. (1983). Assumptions and ambiguities in mechanistic mental models. In Gentner and Stevens, 1983, pages 155–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., and Plunkett, K. (1998). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R. A. and Shepard, R. N. (1986). Visual functions of mental imagery. In Boff, K. R., Kaufman, L., and Thomas, J. P., editors, Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, pages 37.1–37.55. John Wiley & Sons, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought. Thomas Y. Crowell Company, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, N. and Tversky, B. (1990). Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 119:63–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973). The Interpretation of Cultures. Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., Brem, S., Ferguson, R., and Wolff, P. (1997). Analogical reasoning and conceptual change: A case study of Johannes Kepler. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6:3–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. and Gentner, D. R. (1983). Flowing waters and teeming crowds: Mental models of electricity. In Gentner and Stevens, 1983, pages 99–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. and Stevens, A. L., editors (1983). Mental Models. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (1988). Explaining Science: A Cognitive Approach. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N., editor (1992). Cognitive Models of Science. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly, K. J. (1986). Mental modeling: A framework for the study of thinking. In Bishop, J., Lochhead, J., and Perkins, D., editors, Thinking: Progress in Research and Teaching, pages 19–32. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20:1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. (1990). Experiment and the Making of Meaning: Human Agency in Scientific Observation and Experiment. Kluwer, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. (1992). The procedural turn: Or why did Faraday's thought experiments work? In Giere, 1992, pages 45–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. and James, F. A. J. L., editors (1985). Faraday Rediscovered. Stockton Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1989a). A perspective on thinking. American Psychologist, 44:134–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1989b). Situations, mental models, and generative knowledge. In Klahr, D. and Kotovsky, K., editors, Complex information processing, pages 285–318. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griesemer, J. R. (1991). Must scientific diagrams be eliminable? The case of path analysis. Biology and Philosophy, 6:177–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griesemer, J. R. and Wimsatt, W. (1989). Picturing Weismannism: A case study of conceptual evolution. In Ruse, M., editor, What the Philosophy of Biology is: Essays for David Hull, pages 75–137. Klwuer, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, T. W., Nersessian, N., and Goel, A. (1996). The role of generic models in conceptual change. In Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Hillsdale, NJ. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M. (1992). Mental animation: Inferring motion from static diagrams of mechanical systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18:1084–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M. and Just, M. A. (1989). Understanding machines from text and diagrams. In Mandl, H. and Levin, J., editors, Knowledge Acquisition from Text and Picture. North Holland, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M. and Just, M. A. (1994). Constructing mental models of machines from text and diagrams. Journal of Memory and Language, 32:717–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., and Thagard, P. A. (1986). Induction: Processes of Inference, Learning, and Discovery. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). The mental representation of the meaning of words. Cognition, 25:189–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental Models. MIT, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1989). Mental models. In Posner, M., editor, Foundations of Cognitive Science, pages 469–500. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Byrne, R. (1993). Precis of the book, deduction with peer review commentaries and responses. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 16:323–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and Mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and Brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1987). Science in Action. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1999). Pandora's Hope: Essays on the Reality of Science Studies. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. and Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory Life: The Construction of Scientific Facts. Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in Practice: Mind, Mathematics, and Culture in Everyday Life. Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (1985). Art and Artifact in Laboratory Science: A Study of Shop Work and Shop Talk in a Research Laboratory. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. and Woolgar, S., editors (1990). Representation in Scientific Practice. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mani, K. and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). The mental representation of spatial descriptions. Memory and Cognition, 10:181–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. C. (1855). On Faraday's lines of force. Scientific Papers, 1:155–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. C. (1890). The Scientific Papers of James Clerk Maxwell. Cambridge University, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P. and Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Mental models of word meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22:449–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1984). Faraday to Einstein: Constructing Meaning in Scientific Theories. Martinus Nijhoff/Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1985). Faraday's field concept. In Gooding and James, 1985, pages 377–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1988). Reasoning from imagery and analogy in scientific concept formation. In Fine, A. and Leplin, J., editors, PSA 1988, pages 41–47. Philosophy of Science Association, East Lansing, MI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1992a). How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. In Giere, 1992, pages 3–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1992b). In the theoretician's laboratory: Thought experimenting as mental modeling. In Hull, D., Forbes, M., and Okruhlik, K., editors, PSA 1992, pages 291–301. Philosophy of Science Association, East Lansing, MI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1995). Opening the black box: Cognitive science and the history of science. In Thackray, A., editor, Constructing Knowledge in the History of Science, volume 10 of Osiris, pages 194–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1999). Model-based reasoning in conceptual change. In Magnani, L., Nersessian, N. J., and Thagard, P., editors, Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery, pages 5–22. Kluwer/Plenum, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2002). Maxwell and the “method of physical analogy”: Model-based reasoning, generic abstraction, and conceptual change. In Malamet, D., editor, Reading Natural Philosophy: Essays in the History and Philosophy of Science and Mathematics, pages 129–165. Open Court, Lacily, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. and Simon, H. A. (1972). Human Problem Solving. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic v. analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108:291–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1988). The Psychology of Everyday Things. Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norton, J. (1991). Thought experiments in Einstein's work. In Horowitz, T. and Massey, G., editors, Thought Experiments in Science and Philosophy, pages 129–148. Rowman and Littlefield, Savage, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. and Garnham, A., editors (1996). Mental Models in Cognitive Science: Essays in honor of Philip Johnson-Laird. Psychology Press, Philadelphia, Brighton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrig, W. and Kintsch, W. (1985). Propositional and situational representations of text. Journal of Memory and Language, 24:503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. (1981). The imagery debate: Analog media vs. tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 88:16–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. (2001). Visual indexes, preconceptual objects and situated vision. Cognition, 80:127–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B., Levine, J. M., and Teasley, S., editors (1991). Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition. APA Press, Washington DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L. (1986). Mental muddles. In Brand, H. and Hernish, R., editors, The Representation of Knowledge and Belief, pages 258–286. University of Arizona Press, Tuscon, AZ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. and Lloyd, B. (1978). Cognition and Categorization. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L. and Black, J. B. (1996a). Analog imagery in mental model reasoning: Depictive models. Cognitive Psychology, 30:154–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L. and Black, J. B. (1996b). Shuttling between depictive models and abstract rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20:457–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shelley, C. (1996). Visual abductive reasoning in archeology. Philosophy of Science, 63:278–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shelley, C. (1999). Multiple analogies in evolutionary biology. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 30:143–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. (1988). Imagination of the scientist. In Egan, K. and Nadaner, D., editors, Imagination and the Scientist, pages 153–185. Teachers College Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91:417–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. and Cooper, L. A. (1932). Mental Images and their Transformations. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. (1997). Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture and the Problem of Meaning. Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1977). Models of Thought. Reidel, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1981). The Sciences of the Artificial. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human-Machine Communication. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. (1991). Conceptual Revolutions. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tweney, R. D. (1985). Faraday's discovery of induction: A cognitive approach. In Gooding and James, 1985, pages 189–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tweney, R. D. (1992). Stopping time: Faraday and the scientific creation of perceptual order. Physis, 29:149–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Kolk, B., McFarlane, A. C., and Weisaeth, L., editors (1996). Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Overwhelming Experience on Mind, Body, and Society. Guilford Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vera, A. and Simon, H. (1993). Situated cognition: A symbolic interpretation. Cognitive Science, 17:4–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32:109–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. (1968). On the failure…—A second look. In Watson, P. C. and Johnson-Laird, P.N., editors, Thinking and Reasoning, pages 307–314. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, L. P. (1964). Michael Faraday: A Biography. Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, D. D. (1997). Towards a theoretical base for representation design in the computer medium: Ecological perception and aiding human cognition. In Flach, J., Hancock, P., Caird, J., and Vincente, K., editors, The Ecology of Human: Machine Systems, pages 157–188. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeh, W. and Barsalou, L. W. (1996). The role of situations in concept learning. In Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. (1997). The nature of external representations in problem solving. Cognitive Science, 21:179–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. and Norman, D. A. (1995). A representational analysis of numeration systems. Cognition, 57:217–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nancy J. Nersessian .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Nersessian, N.J. (2009). Conceptual Change: Creativity, Cognition, and Culture. In: Meheus, J., Nickles, T. (eds) Models of Discovery and Creativity. Origins: Studies in the Sources of Scientific Creativity, vol 3. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3421-2_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics