Advertisement

Awareness during Anaesthesia

  • V. A. Peduto
Conference paper

Abstract

A patient agreeing to have an operation performed under general anaesthesia expects to be unconscious and free from pain or unpleasant sensations during the surgical procedure. In fact, the introduction of muscle relaxants into anaesthetic practice made it possible for patients to be kept immobile while conscious. The advantages of light anaesthesia for major surgery became apparent, with much less circulatory depression, so it was adopted. However, the signs of anaesthesia were blurred, up to extinction, and a complication arose: “conscious awareness”. Patients can be completely aware during the whole operation or, more frequently, part of it; they may be oriented in time and space, realize that the operation is in progress, feel pain and other sensations, and hear conversation — a horrific experience.

Keywords

Nitrous Oxide Anaesthetic Agent Implicit Memory Explicit Memory Conscious Awareness 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Ghoneim MM, Block RI (1992) Learning and consciousness during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 76:279–305PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Liu WHD, Thorp TAS, Graham SG et al (1991) Incidence of awareness with recall during general anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 46:435–437PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Sandin R, Nordstrom O (1993) Awareness during total i.v. anaesthesia. Brit J Anaesth 71: 782–787PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE (1970) A simple study of awareness and dreaming during anaesthesia. Brit J Anaesth 42:535–541PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Richardson JTE (1989) Human memory: psychology, pathology and pharmacology. Baillière’s Clin Anaesthesiol 3:451–471CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Kihlstrom JF, Schacter DL (1990) Anaesthesia, amnesia, and the cognitive unconscious. In: Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K (eds) Memory and awareness in anaesthesia. Sweets &; Zeitlinger, Amsterdam, pp 21–44Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Prys-Roberts C (1987) Anaesthesia: a practical or unpractical construct? Brit J Anaesth 59: 1341–1345PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    White DC (1987) Anaesthesia: a privation of the sense. An historical introduction and some definitions. In: Rosen M, Lunn JN (eds) Consciousness, awareness and pain in general anaesthesia. Butterworths, London, pp 1–9Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Jessop J, Jones JG (1992) Evaluation of the actions of general anesthetics in the human brain. Gen Pharmacol 23:927–935PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Dixon NF (1989) Unconscious perception and general anaesthesia. Baillière’s Clin Anaesthesiol 3:473–485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Blacher RS (1975) On awakening paralyzed during surgery. A syndrome of traumatic neurosis. JAMA 234:67–68PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Wilson SL, Vaughan RW, Stephen CR (1975) Awareness, dreams, and hallucinations associated with general anesthesia. Anesth Analg 54:609–617PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Lyons G, MacDonald R (1991) Awareness during Caesarean section. Anaesthesia 46:62–64PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Farnsworth GM (1978) Enflurane and the incidence of awareness in Caesarean section. Anaesthesia 33:55Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Heneghan C (1993) Clinical and medicolegal aspects of conscious awareness during anesthesia. Int Anesthesiol Clin 31 (4): 1 –11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Tunstall ME, Sheikh A (1989) Comparison of 1.5% enflurane with 1.25% isoflurane in oxygen for Caesarean section: avoidance of awareness without nitrous oxide. Brit J Anaesth 62:138–143PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Sear JW (1983) General kinetic and dynamic principles and their application to continuous intravenous anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 38:10–25PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Tammisto T, Tigerstedt I (1977) The need for halothane supplementation of N20–02 relaxant anaesthesia in chronic alcoholics. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 21:17–23PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Lemmens HJM, Bovill JG, Hennis PJ et al (1989) Alcohol consumption alters the pharmacodynamics of alfentanil. Anesthesiology 71:669–674PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Sia RL (1969) Consciousness during general anesthesia. Anesth Analg 48:363–366PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Hargrove RL (1989) Awareness: a medicolegal problem. In: Crul JF (ed) Legal aspects of anaesthesia. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, pp 1–6Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Lunn JN, Rosen M (1990) Anaesthetic awareness (correspondence). Brit Med J 300:938PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Ranta S, Jussila J, Hynynen M (1996) Recall of awareness during cardiac anaesthesia: influence of feedback information to the anaesthesiologist. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 40: 554–560PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Tunstall ME (1977) Detecting wakefulness during general anaesthesia. Brit Med J 1:1321PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Russell IF (1986) Comparison of wakefulness with two anaesthetic regimes. Br J Anaesth 58:965–968PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Tunstall ME (1982) Clinical curio: sleep phobia after awareness during general anaesthesia: treatment by induced wakefulness. Brit Med J 285:865Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Artusio JF (1955) Ether analgesia during major surgery. JAMA 157:33–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Levinson BW (1965) States of awareness during general anaesthesia. Brit J Anaesth 37: 544–546PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Mingay DJ (1985–1986) Hypnosis and memory for incidentally learned scenes. Br J Clin Exp Hypnosis 3:173–183Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Stolzy S, Couture LJ, Edmonds HL (1986) Evidence of partial recall during general anesthesia. Anesth Analg 65:S154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Stolzy S, Couture LJ, Edmonds HL (1987) A post-operative recognition test after balanced anesthesia. Anesth Analg 67:A377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Eich E, Reeves JL, Katz RL (1985) Anesthesia, awareness and the memory/awareness distinction. Anesth Analg 64:1143–1148PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Bennett HL, Davis HS, Giannini JA (1985) Non verbal response to intraoperative conversation. Brit J Anaesth 57:174–179PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST et al (1991) Human learning during general anaesthesia. Brit J Anaesth 66:170–178PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Jansen CK, Bonke B, Klein J et al (1990) Unconscious perception during balanced anaesthesia? In: Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K (eds) Memory and awareness in anaesthesia. Sweets & Zeitlinger, Amsterdam, pp 115–119Google Scholar
  36. 36.
    Bonke B, Schmitz PIM, Verhage F et al (1986) Clinical study of so-called unconscious perception during anaesthesia. Brit J Anaesth 58:957–964PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Bocke S, Bonke B, Bouwhuis-Hoogerwerf ML et al (1988) Effects of sounds presented during general anaesthesia on postoperative course. Brit J Anaesth 60:697–702CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Woo R, Seltzer JL, Marr A (1987) The lack of response to suggestion under controlled surgical anaesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 31:567–571PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Evans C, Richardson PH (1988) Improved recovery and reduced post-operative stay after therapeutic suggestions during general anaesthesia. Lancet 2:491–493PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.
    McLintock TTC, Aitken H, Downic CFA et al (1990) Postoperative analgesic requirements in patients exposed to positive intraoperative suggestions. Brit Med J 301:788–790PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Schwender D, Klasing S, Madler C et al (1993) Midlatency auditory evoked potentials and cognitive function during general anesthesia. Int Anesthesiol Clin 31(4): 89–106PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. 42.
    Savoia G, Esposito C, Belfiore F et al (1988) Propofol infusion and auditory evoked potentials. Anaesthesia 43:46–49PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Barker SJ (1995) Positron Emission Tomography suggests the functional neuroanatomy of implicit memory during propofol anaesthesia. Abst 3rd International Symposium on Memory and Awareness in Anaesthesia, Rotterdam, June 8–10Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Thornton C (1991) Evoked potentials in anaesthesia. Eur J Anaesthesiol 8:89–107PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Chortkoff BS, Bennett HL, Eger EI II (1993) Subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane suppress learning as defined by the category–example task. Anesthesiology 79:16–22PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Italia 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • V. A. Peduto

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations