The Gestural Origins of Language

  • Michael C. Corballis


The idea that language evolved from manual gestures dates at least to the philosopher (1971/1746), but was revived in modern format by (1973). The idea was controversial at the time, and remains so, but it continues to be advocated, and appears to have gained increasing acceptance (e.g., Arbib 2005; Armstrong 1999; Armstrong et al. 1995; Corballis 2002; Givòn 1979; Rizzolatti and Arbib 1998; Ruben 2005). From an evolutionary point of view, the idea makes some sense, since nonhuman primates have little if any cortical control over vocalization, but excellent cortical control over the hands and arms. Attempts over the past half-century to teach our closest nonhuman relatives, the great apes, to speak have been strikingly unsuccessful, but relatively good progress has been made toward teaching them to communicate by a form of sign language (Gardner and Gardner 1969), or by using visual symbols on a keyboard (Savage-Rumbaugh et al. 1998). These visual forms of communication scarcely resemble the grammatical language of modern humans, but they are a considerable advance over the paucity of speech sounds that these animals can make. The human equivalents of primate vocalizations are probably emotionally-based sounds like laughing, crying, grunting, or shrieking, rather than words.


Sign Language American Sign Mirror System Language Disorder FOXP2 Gene 


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Ajello R, Mazzoni L, Nicolai F (2001) Linguistic gestures: Mouthing in Italian Sign Languages (LIS). In: Sutton-Spence R, Boyes-Braem P (eds) The hands are the head of the mouth: The mouth as articulator in sign language. Signum-Verlag, Hamburg, pp 231–246Google Scholar
  2. Arbib MA (2005) From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 28:105–168Google Scholar
  3. Armstrong DF (1999) Original signs: Gesture, sign, and the source of language. Gallaudet University Press, WashingtonGoogle Scholar
  4. Armstrong DF, Stokoe WC, Wilcox SE (1995) Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  5. Bergman B, Wallin L (2001) A preliminary analysis of visual mouth segments in Swedish Sign Language. In: Sutton-Spence R, Boyes-Braem P (eds). The hands are the head of the mouth: The mouth as articulator in sign language. Signum-Verlag, Hamburg, pp 51–68Google Scholar
  6. Bickerton D (1995) Language and human behavior. University of Washington Press, Seattle, WAGoogle Scholar
  7. Boutla M, Supalla T, Newport EL, Bavelier D (2004) Short-term memory span: Insights from sign language. Nature Neuroscience 7:997–1002PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Browman CP, Goldstein LF (1995) Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In: van Gelder T, Port RF (eds) Mind as motion. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 175–193Google Scholar
  9. Buccino G, Lui F, Canessa N, Patteri I, Lagravinese G, Benuzzi F, Porro CA, Rizzolatti G (2004) Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16:114–126PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Burling R (2005) The talking ape. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  11. Calvert GA, Campbell R (2003) Reading speech from still and moving faces: The neural substrates of visible speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15:57–70PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. de Condillac EB (1971) An essay on the origin of human knowledge. T. Nugent (Tr.). Scholars Facsimiles and Reprints, Gainesville (Originally published 1746)Google Scholar
  13. Corballis MC (1991) The lopsided ape. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Corballis MC (2002) From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJGoogle Scholar
  15. Corballis MC (2003) From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 26:199–260Google Scholar
  16. Corballis MC (2004) The origins of modernity: Was autonomous speech the critical factor? Psychological Review 111:543–522PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Crow TJ (2002) Sexual, selection, timing, and an X-Y homologous gene: Did Homo sapiens speciate on the Y chromosome? In: Crow TJ (ed) The speciation of modern Homo sapiens. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp 197–216Google Scholar
  18. DeGusta D, Gilbert WH, Turner SP (1999) Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96:1800–1804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Emmorey K (2002) Language, cognition, and brain: Insights from sign language research. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  20. Emmorey K (2005) Sign languages are problematic for a gestural origins theory of language evolution. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 28:130–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Wiebe V, Kitano T, Monaco AP, Paabo S (2002) Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418:869–871PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, Pembrey ME (1998) Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genet 18:168–170PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gardner RA, Gardner BT (1969) Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165:664–672PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gibson KR, Jessee S (1999) Language evolution and expansions of multiple neurological processing areas. In: King BJ (ed) The origins of language: What nonhuman primates can tell us. School of American Research Press, Santa Fe, NM, pp 189–228Google Scholar
  25. Givòn T (1979) On understanding grammar. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  26. Goldin-Meadow S, McNeill D (1999) The role of gesture and mimetic representation in making language the province of speech. In: Corballis MC, Lea SEG (eds) The descent of mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 155–172Google Scholar
  27. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  28. Gopnik M (1990) Feature-blind grammar and dysphasia. Nature 344:715PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Haldane JBS (1927) A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection, part V: Selection and mutation. Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 23:838–844CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hewes GW (1973) Primate communication and the gestural origins of language. Current Anthropology 14:5–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Joos M (1948) Acoustic phonetics. Language Monograph No. 23. Linguistic Society of America, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
  32. Kay RF, Cartmill M, Barlow M (1998) The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95:5417–5419PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Knight A, Underhill PA, Mortensen HM, Zhivotovsky LA, Lin AA, Henn BM, Louis D, Ruhlen M, Mountain JL (2003) African Y chromosome and mtDNA divergence provides insight into the history of click languages. Current Biology 13:464–473PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kohler E, Keysers C, Umilta MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2002) Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297:846–848PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Konner M (1982) The tangled wing: biological constraints on the human spirit. Harper, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  36. Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP (2001) A novel forkheaddomain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413:519–523PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Liberman AM, Cooper FS, Shankweiler DP, Studdert-Kennedy M (1967) Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74:431–461PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Liberman DE (1998) Sphenoid shortening and the evolution of modern cranial shape. Nature 393:158–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lieberman DE, McBratney BM, Krovitz G (2002) The evolution and development of cranial form in Homo sapiens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99:1134–1139.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lieberman P (1998) Eve spoke: Human language and human evolution. W.W. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. Lieberman P, Crelin ES, Klatt DH (1972) Phonetic ability and related anatomy of the new-born, adult human, Neanderthal man, and the chimpanzee. American Anthropologist 74:287–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Liégeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F (2003) Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neuroscience 6:1230–1237PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. MacLarnon A, Hewitt G (1999) The evolution of human speech: The role of enhanced breathing control. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 109:341–363PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. MacLarnon A, Hewitt G (2004) Increased breathing control: Another factor in the evolution of human language. Evolutionary Anthropology 13:181–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Marcus GF, Fisher SE (2003) FOXP2 in focus: What can genes tell us about speech and language? Trends in Cognitive Science 7:257–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. McBrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior: Journal of Human Evolution 39:453–563PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McGurk H, MacDonald J (1976) Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264:746–748PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McNeill D (1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  49. Mellars P (2006) Going east: New genetic and archaeological perspectives on the modern human colonization of Eurasia. Science 313:796–800PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mellars PA, Stringer CB (eds) (1989) The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans. Edinburgh University Press, EdinburghGoogle Scholar
  51. Muir LJ, Richardson IEG (2005) Perception of sign language and its application to visual communications for deaf people. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education 10:390–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Neidle C, Kegl J, MacLaughlin D, Bahan B, Lee RG (2000) The syntax of American Sign Language. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  53. Olivieri A, Achilli A, Pala M, Battaglia V, Fornarino S, Al-Zaherym N, Scozzari R, Cruciani F, Behar DM, Dugoujon JM, Coudray C, Santachiara-Benerecotti AS, Semino O, Bandelt HJ, Torroni A (2006) The mtDNA legacy of the Levantine Early Upper Palaeolithic in Africa. Science 314:1767–1770PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Oppenheimer S (2003) Out of Eden: The peopling of the world. Constable, LondonGoogle Scholar
  55. Pinker S (1994) The language instinct. Morrow, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  56. Ploog D (2002) Is the neural basis of vocalisation different in non-human primates and Homo sapiens? In: Crow TJ (ed) The speciation of modern Homo Sapiens. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp 121–135Google Scholar
  57. Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21:188–194Google Scholar
  58. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2001) Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews 2: 661–670PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Rogers LJ, Andrew RJ (2002) Comparative vertebrate lateralization. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  60. Ruben RJ (2005) Sign language: Its history and contribution to the understanding of the biological nature of language. Acta Oto-Laryngolica 125:464–467CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Russell BA, Cerny FJ, Stathopoulos ET (1998) Effects of varied vocal intensity on ventilation and energy expenditure in women and men. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41:239–248PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Savage-Rumbaugh S, Shanker SG, Taylor TJ (1998) Apes, language, and the human mind. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  63. Shu WG, Yang HH, Zhang LL, Lu MM, Morrisey EE (2001) Characterization of a new subfamily of winged-helix/forkhead (Fox) genes that are expressed in the lung and act as transcriptional repressors. Journal of Biological Chemistry 276:27488–27497PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Studdert-Kennedy M (1998) The particulate origins of language generativity: From syllable to gesture. In: Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 169–176Google Scholar
  65. Sutton-Spence R, Day L (2001) Mouthings and mouth gestures in British Sign Language. In: Sutton-Spence R, Boyes-Braem P (eds) The hands are the head of the mouth: The mouth as articulator in sign language. Signum-Verlag, Hamburg, pp 69–85Google Scholar
  66. Thorne A, Grün R, Mortimer G, Spooner NA, Simpson JJ, McCulloch M, Taylor L, Curnoe D (1999) Australia’s oldest human remains: Age of the Lake Mungo human skeleton. Journal of Human Evolution 36:591–612PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Alcock KJ, Fletcher P, Passingham R (1995) Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92:930–933PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Watkins KE, Dronkers NF, Vargha-Khadem F (2002a) Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: Comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain 125:452–464PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, Passingham RE, Connelly A, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Mishkin M, Gadian DG (2002b) MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain 125:465–478PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael C. Corballis
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Private Bag 92019University of AucklandAucklandNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations