Advertisement

Two Types of Anticipatory-Timing Mechanisms in Synchronization Tapping

  • Yoshihiro Miyake
  • Yohei Onishi
  • Ernst Pöppel

Abstract

Mutual coordination of timing is required to produce synchronous cooperative behavior between humans, and an anticipation mechanism related to external events is thought to be indispensable to generate such movement. The importance of this timing control becomes clear if one considers, for example, playing together in a musical ensemble. However, it has been reported that a time difference exists between awareness of cognitive synchrony and physical synchrony, such as a negative asynchrony phenomenon (see next paragraph). Analysis of this anticipatory mechanism should be performed, not only to elucidate the physical process, but also to understand the underlying cognitive process in which a higher brain function, such as attention (Kahnemann 1973), is involved.

Keywords

Memory Task Auditory Stimulus Stimulus Onset Secondary Task Attentional Resource 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aschersleben G, Prinz W (1995) Synchronizing actions with events: the role of sensory information. Percept Psychophys 57:305–317PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley A (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley A (1998a) Working memory. Comptes Rendus de l’Academie des Sciences — Series III — Science de la Vie 321:167–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley A (1998b) Recent developments in working memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 8:234–238PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown SW (1997) Attentional resources in timing: interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Percept Psychophys 59:1118–1140PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Daneman M, Carpenter PA (1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 19:450–466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fraisse P (1966) The sensorimotor synchronization of rhythms. In: Requin J (Eds) Anticipation et comportement. Centre National, Paris, pp 233–257Google Scholar
  8. Ivry RB (1996) The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:851–853PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ivry RB (1997) Neural mechanisms of timing. Trends Cogn Sci 1:163–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Kagerer FA, Wittmann M, Szelag E, Steinbüchel N (2002) Cortical involvement in temporal reproduction: evidence for differential roles of the hemispheres. Neuropsychologia 40:357–366PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Kahnemann D (1973) Attention and efforts. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs NJGoogle Scholar
  12. Kawato M (1996) Computational Theory of Brain (In Japanese). Sangyo Tosho Publisher, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  13. Kolers PA, Brewster JM (1985) Rhythms and responses. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:150–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. LaBerge D, Samuels SJ (1974) Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognit Psychol 6:293–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Laberge D (1975) Acquisition of automatic processing of perceptual and associative learning. In: Rabbitt PMA, Dornic S (Eds) Attention and performance V. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. Macar R, Casini L (1999) Multiple approaches to investigate the existence of an internal clock using attentional resources. Behav Process 45:73–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mangles JA, Ivry RB, Shimizu N (1998) Dissociable contributions of the prefrontal and neocerebellar cortex to time perception. Cogn Brain Res 7:15–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mates J, Radil T, Müler U, Pöppel E (1994) Temporal integration in sensorimotor synchronization. J Cogn Neurosci 6:332–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Miyake Y, Heiss J, Pöppel E (2001) Dual-anticipation in sensory-motor synchronization. Proceedings of 1st Int. Symp. on Measurement, Analysis and Modeling of Human Functions (ISHF2001), Sapporo, Japan, pp 61–66Google Scholar
  20. Miyake Y, Onishi Y, Pöppel E (2002) Two modes of timing anticipation in synchronization tapping (in Japanese). Transaction of SICE 38:1114–1122Google Scholar
  21. Miyake Y, Onishi Y, Pöppel E (2004) Two types of anticipation in synchronous tapping. Acta Neurobiol Exp 64:415–426Google Scholar
  22. Osaka N (2000) Brain and working memory. Kyoto University Press, KoyotoGoogle Scholar
  23. Osaka M, Osaka N (1994) Working memory capacity related to reading: measurement with the Japanese version of reading span test. Jpn J Psychol 65:339–345Google Scholar
  24. Pascual-Leone A (2001) Increased variability of paced finger tapping accuracy following repetitive magnetic stimulation of the cerebellum in humans. Neurosci Lett 306:29–32PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pearson K (1976) The control of walking. Sci Am 235:72–86PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Peters M (1989) The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration. Psychol Res 51:38–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pöppel E (1971) Oscillation as possible basis for time perception. Studium Generale 24:85–107PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Pöppel E (1988) Mind works: time and conscious experience. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Boston MAGoogle Scholar
  29. Pöppel E (1997) A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends Cogn Sci 1: 56–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Rao SM, Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Bobholz JA, Cox RW, Binder JR (1997) Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. J Neurosci 17:5528–5535PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Saitoh S (1993) The disappearance of the phonological similarity effect by complex rhythmic tapping. Psychologia 36:27–33Google Scholar
  32. Saitoh S (1997) Research of phonetic working memory (in Japanese). Fuhma Shobo Publisher, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  33. Stevens LT (1886) On the time sense. Mind 11:393–404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Szelag E, Kowalska J, Rymarczyk K, Pöppel E (2002) Duration processing in children as determined by time reproduction: implications for a few seconds temporal window. Acta Psychol 110:1–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Woodrow H (1932) The effect of rate of sequence upon the accuracy of synchronization. J Exp Psychol 15:357–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Zelaznik HN, Spencer RMC, Ivry RV (2002) Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28:575–588PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Yoshihiro Miyake
    • 1
  • Yohei Onishi
    • 1
  • Ernst Pöppel
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems ScienceTokyo Institute of TechnologyMidori, YokohamaJapan
  2. 2.Institute of Medical PsychologyLudwig-Maximilian University of MunichMunichGermany

Personalised recommendations