Skip to main content

Herder on “Sentio, Ergo Sum”: Seen from His Remarks on the Color Harpsichord

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Applied Sciences and Technology ((BRIEFSAPPLSCIENCES))

Abstract

This chapter outlines the unique aesthetics of German philosopher Johann Gottfried Herder (1744–1803) by paying attention to his remarks on the “color harpsichord”. “Color harpsichord” is a curious instrument invented by French Jesuit Louis-Bertrand Castel (1688–1757), inspired by the correlation theory between spectral hues and musical scale which English natural scientist Isaac Newton (1642–1726) proposed in Opticks (1704). So Castel attempted to visualize this correspondence between spectrum and musical scale by conceiving an instrument that, on striking a key, shows a color corresponding to the musical note. This color harpsichord was discussed heatedly by many great philosophers of the time (Rousseau, Diderot, Goethe etc.). Herder is among them the one who was most interested in (mentioned most times) the color harpsichord, though anytime negatively. He took the failure of the color harpsichord because of lack of doubt or reflection on sight and hearing being modally different and forces an unnatural mode of perception in reverse of the process of human development, from a “sensorium commune” to a being with five differentiated senses. This argument can be summarized, as Herder himself wrote, into the motto “sentio, ergo sum”, as opposed to the Cartesian “cogito, ergo sum”.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In Sugiyama (2009), I have already argued for Herder as the pioneer of a philosophical concept of synesthesia, but without referring to the color organ.

  2. 2.

    For Castel’s biography, see Schier (1941), Chouillet-Roche (1976), Franssen (1991).

  3. 3.

    Yet, Castel adopted here not the theory of Newton, but that of Descartes, who in Dioptrics (Dioptrique, 1637) mentioned blue, yellow, and red as basic (Descartes 1996, VI 92) and correlated them to the root, the third, and the fifth of triad. In order to make more suitable for the organ, he further correlated not seven colors to the diatonic, but twelve to the chromatic scale (Castel 1740, 173).

  4. 4.

    Rousseau said, “I have seen the famous clavichord on which music is supposedly made with colors” (Rousseau 1959–1996, V 419 = 61f.), but without detailed information regarding when, where, and how it was exhibited.

  5. 5.

    For the first performance (21.12.1755), “I gave an invitation to fifty persons of the house and from abroad, and lighted a hundred wax candles. There was nothing but acclamation and clapping of hands in consequence for the space of half an hour that I played” (Anonymous 1757, 12). For the second performance, “Every one wanted to see, and see again this novelty. […] The Harpsichord played, and two hundred persons owned that they had never seen any thing more beautiful, or more brilliant, and all concluded that the Harpsichord was executable, possible, true, and even made” (13).

  6. 6.

    For the difference between them, see Jewanski (1999, 479).

  7. 7.

    Henceforth I refer to, but do not always follow for the sake of unity, Herder (2002a), Herder (2002b), and Herder (2006) as the English translations of Herder’s writings.

  8. 8.

    Although “force (Kraft)” is a problematic concept, Herder explained it just before the quotation above: “Painting operates in space and by means of an artful representation of space. Music and all energetic arts operate not just in but also through time, through an artful succession of tones. Would it not be possible to reduce the essence of poetry as well to such a primary concept, since it achieves its effect on the soul through arbitrary signs, through meaning of word? We shall call the means of this effect force” (194). For this concept in the context of aesthetics (focusing on Herder), see Menke (2012).

  9. 9.

    Herder said that this book was “largely written in the years 1768–70” (244). Fourth Grove of Critical Forests, which was written during this time and published posthumously, contains therefore the corresponding contents and is, in a sense, a study work for Sculpture. For this argument see also II 307.

  10. 10.

    See also II 325

  11. 11.

    See also II 295f.. For the problem in general, see e.g. Morgan (1977).

  12. 12.

    For this point, see Sugiyama (2009).

  13. 13.

    Mendelssohn, whose close dialogue with Lessing produced Laocoon, said, for example: “It is indisputable that we are able to discriminate far more colors than sounds in the same time. For experience teaches that each individual color still sustains itself within the eye for a while if, at the same time, we have closed our eyes. Hence, in a melody of colors the impression which the foregoing colors have left behind them must be mixed with the present ones and produce an effect completely different from the one demanded. The nerves of the sense of hearing appear to retain the impression for a shorter time if the sound does not cause a very large vibration in the air. Indeed, although it may be hoped that, by habitualizing the nerves of the eye, one could even bring things to this point (since in the case of hearing itself it is very much a matter of habit), at least at the outset one would still have to let the colors follow upon one another more slowly and after lengthier intervals than do sounds, and only after long practice be mindful of a unification of the melody of the colors with the melody of sounds. / Father Castel first came up with the idea of putting a melody of colors into practice, and Krüger improved this invention considerably. But neither must have thought of this difficulty” (Mendelssohn 1929ff., I 117). It seems noteworthy here that Mendelssohn referred to “(a matter of) habit (Gewohnheit)” and “practice (Übung)”. Therefore, he might have thought it possible in future—though impossible in his time—to accept (the mode of perception based on) the color organ without awkwardness, given long practice or habitualization of the nerves, whereas Herder thought it embryologically impossible.

  14. 14.

    See e.g. Cytowic (1993, 73).

References

  • Anonymous (1757) Explanation of the ocular harpsichord, upon shew to the public, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Caduff C (2002) Fantom Farbenklavier. Das Farbe-Ton-Verhältnis im 18. Jahrhundert oder vom Einspruch gegen das clavecin oculaire und seinen ästhetischen Folgen. Zeitschrift für deutsche Philologie 121:481–509

    Google Scholar 

  • Castel L-B (1723) Traité d’optique … par M. le Chevalier Newton; traduit par M. Coste. Journal de Trévoux 1723:1428–1450

    Google Scholar 

  • Castel L-B (1725) Clavecin pour les yeux, avec l’art de peindre les sons et toutes sortes de pièce de musique. Mercure de France 1725:2552–2577

    Google Scholar 

  • Castel L-B (1735) Nouvelles expériences d’optiques et et d’acoustique. Journal de Trévoux 1735:1444ff

    Google Scholar 

  • Castel L-B (1740) L’optique des couleurs, fondée sur les simples observations et tournée surtout à la pratique de la peinture avec figures. Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Chouillet-Roche A-M (1976) Le clavecin oculaire du père Castel. Dix-huitième siècle 8:141–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cytowic RE (1993) The man who tasted shapes. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio A (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio A (2000) Ich fühle, also bin ich. Die Entschlüsselung des Bewusstseins. Übers. von Hainer Kober. München: List

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes R 1996 (11897–1913). Œuvres. 12 vols. Publiées par Charles Adam & Paul Tannery. Nouvelle éd. Vrin, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Franssen M (1991) The ocular harpsichord of Louis-Bertrand Castel. The science and aesthetics of an eighteenth-century cause célèbre. Tractrix 3:15–77

    Google Scholar 

  • Herder JG 1985–2000. Werke in zehn Bänden. Hg. von Günter Arnold et al. Deutscher Klassiker, Frankfurt am Main

    Google Scholar 

  • Herder JG (2002a) Philosophical Writings (ed and trans, Forster M). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Herder JG (2002b) Sculpture: some observations on shape and form from Pygmalion’s creative dream (ed and trans, Gaiger J). University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Herder JG (2006) Selected writings of aesthetics (ed and trans, Moore G). Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Jewanski J (1999) Ist C = rot? Eine Kultur- und Wissenschaftsgeschichte zum Problem der wechselseitigen Beziehung zwischen Ton und Farbe von Aristoteles bis Goethe. Studio, Sinzig

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger JG (1740–1749) Naturlehre. 3 Bde., Halle

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger JG (1743) De novo musices, quo oculi delectantur, genere. Miscellanea Berolinensia ad incrementum scientiarum ex scriptis societati regiae scientiarum 7:345–357

    Google Scholar 

  • Lessing GE (1968) (11886–1924) Sämtliche Schriften. 23 Bde. Hg. von Karl Lachmann. ND. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendelssohn M (1929) Gesammelte Schriften. Jubiläumsausgabe. Akademie, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Menke C (2012) Force: fundamental concept of aesthetic anthropology (trans, Jackson G). Fordham Univeristy Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan MJ (1977) Molyneux’s question: vision, touch and the philosophy of perception. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau J-J (1959–1996) Œuvres complètes, 5 vols. Gallimard, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Schier DS (1941) Louis Bertrand Castel, anti-Newtonian scientist. Torch Press, Cedar Rapids (IA)

    Google Scholar 

  • Sugiyama T (2009) Herder’s theory of common sense: the birth of the concept of synesthesia. Aesthetics 13:69–81. http://www.bigakukai.jp/aesthetics_online/aesthetics_13/text/text13_sugiyama.pdf. Accessed 30 June 2014

  • Telemann GP (1739) Beschreibung der Augen-Orgel oder des Augen-Clavicimbels, so der berühmte Mathematicus und Jesuit zu Paris, Herr Pater Castel, erfunden und ins Werk gerichtet hat. Hamburg

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Takashi Sugiyama .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Sugiyama, T. (2019). Herder on “Sentio, Ergo Sum”: Seen from His Remarks on the Color Harpsichord. In: Computational Aesthetics. SpringerBriefs in Applied Sciences and Technology. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-56844-5_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics