Skip to main content

Evolutionary Locus of the Neanderthal Between Chimpanzees and Modern Humans: A Working Memory, Theory of Mind, and Brain Developmental, Piagetian Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers

Part of the book series: Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series ((RNMH))

  • 1037 Accesses

Abstract

A hypothetical psychological learning model of the Neanderthals is proposed by interpolating the Neanderthals on a genetic, evolutional, and developmental continuum between chimpanzees and modern humans based upon five empirical results found by anthropological, behavioral, genetic, and cognitive psychological studies. The model claims that the Neanderthals had limited working memory (WM) capacity, had a limited theory of mind (ToM), and were at the preoperational stage. The main conclusion is that it is the ability to teach and to learn through being taught that discriminates modern humans from the Neanderthals because teaching behavior prevails even in hunter-gatherers and teaching motivation has an evolutionary basis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Akagi K (2004) Can one year-olds teach?: active teaching when observing others who cannot solve problems. Jpn J Dev Psychol 15(3):366–375

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando J (2009) Evolutionary and genetic bases of education: an adaptive perspective. Annu Rep Educ Psychol Jpn 48:235–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando J (2012a) Is educational behavior evolutionary product or cultural product?: an example of “educational behavior” of hunter-gatherer, Baka pigmy. Paper presented at the 54th annual conference of Japanese society of educational psychology. Ryukyu University, Okinawa, 23 Nov 2012

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando J (2012b) On “Homo educans” hypothesis. In: Watanabe S (ed) CARLS series of advanced study of logic and sensibility. Keio University Press, Tokyo, pp 147–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando J (2013) A bridge between cognitive ability and personality: a longitudinal (14 year) twin study of working memory and GFP. Paper presented at the 54th annual meeting of Japan society of social psychology, Okinawa International University, Okinawa, 3 June 1998

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando J (2014) Comparisons between individual, imitative and instructed learning. In: Akazawa T, Ogihara N, Tanabe H, Terashima H (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol 2, Cognitive and physical perspectives. Springer, Tokyo, pp 17–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando J, Ono Y, Wright M (2001) Genetic structure of spatial and verbal working memory. Behav Genet 31:615–624

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E (2008) Comparing endocranial form and shape differences in modern humans and Neanderthals: a geometric approach. Paleoanthropol 93−106

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, Manzi G, Arsuaga JJ (2003) Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: evidence from the Neanderthal and modern lineages. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100(26):15335–15340

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne RW, Rapaport LG (2011) What are we learning from teaching? Anim Behav 82:1207–1211

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM, Hauser MD (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Q Rev Biol 67:151–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole M (2010) What’s culture got to do with it?: educational research as a necessarily interdisciplinary enterprise. Educ Res 39:461–470

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole M, Brunner JS (1971) Cultural differences and inferences about psychological processes. Am Psychol 26:87–876

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole M, Scribner S (1974) Culture and thought. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2005) Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Camb Archaeol J 15(1):5–26

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne E (2004) Brain development in autism: early overgrowth followed by premature arrest of growth. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 10(2):106–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne E, Carper R, Akshoomoff N (2003) Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. J Am Med Assoc 290:337–344

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan N (2000) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci 24:87–185

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Esposit M, Detre JA, Alsop DC, Shin RK, Atlas S, Grossman M (1995) The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 378(6554):279–281

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B (2000) Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res 133(1):3–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasen PR (1977) Piagetian psychology: cross-cultural contribution. Gardner, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasen PR (1984) The cross-cultural study of intelligence: Piaget and the Boule. Int J Psychol 19:407–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond A (2013) Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 64:131–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson KA, Kintsch W (1995) Long-term working memory. Psychol Rev 102:211–245

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans PD, Gilbert SL, Mekel-Bobrov N, Vallender EJ, Anderson JR, Vaez-Azizi LM, Tishkoff SA, Hudson RR, Lahn BT (2005) Microcephalin, a gene regulating brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans. Science 309(5741):1717–1720

    Google Scholar 

  • Flynn JR (1987) Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: what IQ tests really measure. Psychol Bull 101:171–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Franks NR, Richardson T (2006) Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature 439:153

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman NP, Miyake A, Robinson JL, Hewitt JK (2011) Developmental trajectories in toddlers’ self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: a behavioral genetic analysis. Dev Psychol 47(5):1410–1430

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujisawa KK, Ozaki K, Ozaki K, Yamagata S, Kawahashi I, Ando J (2012) The genetic and environmental relationships between head circumference growth in the first year of life and sociocognitive development in the second year: a longitudinal twin study. Dev Sci 15:99–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodall JVL (1969) My friends the wild chimpanzee. National Geographic Society, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behavior. I, II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter–gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366(1567):1168–1178

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD (2012) Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci 16(3):174–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoppitt WJE, Brown GR, Kendal R, Rendell L, Thornton A, Webster MM, Laland KN (2008) Lessons from animal teaching. Trends Ecol Evol 23(9):486–493

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt E (1974) Quote raven? nevermore. In: Gregg LW (ed) Knowledge and cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishida H (2005) Kaseki ha kataru (Fossils speak). In: Akazwa T (ed) Neanderutahru no shoutai-Karera no ‘nayami’ ni semaru. (Real identity of the Neanderthals: an approach toward their ‘annoyance’. Asahi Shinnbunn Shuppan, Tokyo, pp 165–184

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamei N (2010) Mori no chiisana hantah tachi-shuryou saishuumin no kodomo no minnzokushi (Little hunters in forests: folklore of hunter-gatherer children). Kyoto University Press, Kyoto

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawachi M (2005) Thinking about how they grew. In: Akazwa T (ed) Neanderutahru no shouti-Karera no ‘nayami’ ni semaru. (Real identity of the Neanderthals: an approach toward their ‘annoyance’. Asahi Shinnbunn Shuppan, Tokyo, pp 205–236

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein RG, Edger B (2002) The dawn of human culture. Nevaumont Publishing Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline MA (2015) How to learn about teaching: an evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behav Brain Sci 38:1–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy DL (2010) Learning “from nobody”: the limited role of teaching in folk models of children’s development. Child Past 3:79–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Striano T, Tomasello M (2006) 12 and 18 month olds point to provide information for others. J Cogn Dev 7(2):173–187

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa T (2011) Souzou suru Chikara- Chimpanzee ga oshietekureta ningen no kokoro (An ability to imagine: what chimpanzees teach us about the human mind). Iwanami Shoten, Tokyo

    Google Scholar 

  • Mekel-Bobrov N, Gilbert SL, Evans P, Vallender EJ, Anderson JR, Hudson RR, Tishkoff SA, Lahn BT (2005) Ongoing adaptive evolution of ASPM, a brain size determinant in Homo sapiens. Science 309(5741):1720–1722

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen SJ (1996) The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, religion, and science. Thames and Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen SJ (2005) The singing Neanderthals: the origins of music, language, mind and body. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Mong X, Hashiya K (2014) Pointing behavior in infants reflects the communication partner’s attentional and knowledge states: a possible case of spontaneous informing. PLoS One 9(9):e107579

    Google Scholar 

  • Musek J (2007) A general factor of personality: evidence for the Big One in the five-factor model. J Res Pers 41(6):1213–1233

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak AM, Sigmund K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393:573–577

    Google Scholar 

  • Oda R, Dai M, Niwa Y, Ihobe H, Kiyonari T, Takeda M, Hiraishi H (2013) Self-Report Altruism Scale Distinguished by the Recipient (SRAS-DR): validity and reliability. Jpn J Psychol 84(1):28–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Oesterdiekhoff GW (2012) Was pre-modern man a child? The quintessence of the psychometric and developmental approaches. Intelligence 40:470–478

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone J (1980) Constructive problems for constructive theories: the current relevance of Piaget’s work and a critique of information-processing simulation psychology. In: Kuwe R, Spada H (eds) Developmental models of thinking. Academic, New York, pp 263–296

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1970) Piaget’s theory. In: Mussen PH (ed) Carmichael’s manual of child psychology, vol 1. Wiley, New York, pp 703–732

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack DG, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1(4):515–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Redcay E, Courchesne E (2005) When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biol Psychiatry 58:1–9

    Google Scholar 

  • Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML (1999) The modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT). Self-published, Storrs

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff B (2003) The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton JP, Bons TA, Hur Y-M (2008) The genetics and evolution of the general factor of personality. J Res Pers 42(5):1173–1185

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton JP, Bons TA, Ando J, Hur Y-M, Irwing P, Vernon PA, Petrides KV, Barbaranelli C (2009) A general factor of personality from multitrait-multimethod data and cross-national twins. Twin Res Hum Genet 12:356–365

    Google Scholar 

  • Scribner S, Cole M (1981) The psychology of literacy. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss S, Ziv M (2011) What nonhuman animal teaching teaches us about teaching. In: Watanabe S (ed) CARLS series of advanced studies of logic and sensibility, vol 5. Keio University Press, Tokyo, pp 363–371

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss S, Ziv M (2012) Teaching is a natural cognitive ability for humans. Mind Brain Educ 6(4):186–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton A, McAuliffe K (2006) Teaching in wild meerkats. Science 313:227–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton A, Raihani NJ (2008) The evolution of teaching. Anim Behav 75:1823–1836

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto S, Humle T, Tanaka M (2013) Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique. Plos One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055768

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer C (2006) Evolution: the triumph of an idea. Harper Perennial, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Juko Ando .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Japan

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ando, J. (2016). Evolutionary Locus of the Neanderthal Between Chimpanzees and Modern Humans: A Working Memory, Theory of Mind, and Brain Developmental, Piagetian Perspective. In: Terashima, H., Hewlett, B.S. (eds) Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers. Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_25

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_25

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Tokyo

  • Print ISBN: 978-4-431-55995-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-4-431-55997-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics