Skip to main content

Innovation, Processes of Social Learning, and Modes of Cultural Transmission Among the Chabu Adolescent Forager-Farmers of Ethiopia

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series ((RNMH))

Abstract

Based upon an earlier study of innovation and the transmission and acquisition of innovative skills and knowledge among Aka forager adolescents of central Africa, this study examines the same topic among Chabu adolescents of southwestern Ethiopia, a forager-farming society. Research with the Aka foragers questioned several evolutionary predictions about who should innovate and how innovations should be transmitted (i.e., adolescents, males in particular, should be more innovative than children and adults, innovations should spread by horizontal transmission, and adolescents should pay particular attention to prestigious, “successful” peers). In-depth and structured interviews, informal observations, and systematic ranking and sorting techniques with 14 Chabu adolescents and seven adults were utilized in this study. Major results in terms of the processes and modes of social learning suggest that: (1) oblique modes of transmission were exhibited with greater frequency than horizontal, while prestige bias was indirectly important and linked to reproductive efforts and (2) direct teaching was very important but not exclusively utilized as a means of transmitting social knowledge and reflective of the complexity of skills being taught. Additional results indicate that as with the Aka data, (3) cultural terms specific to innovation existed, (4) innovations and innovators were easily identified by adolescents, (5) innovators were actively sought out individuals who exhibited pro-social qualities and were “good teachers,” and (6) adults were more often identified by the adolescents to be innovators. One result was inconsistent with the Aka study: Chabu adult females were more likely to be identified as innovators than were adult males. As the Chabu women produce highly valued and desired pottery with innovative designs, which sell well at the local market, these data suggest that innovative gender bias is domain dependent.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

References

  • Aoki K (2013) Determinants of cultural evolutionary rates. In: Akazawa T et al (eds) The dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans volume 1: cultural perspectives, replacement of Neanderthals by modern humans series. doi:10.1007/978-4-431-545118_13

  • Barton CM, Riel-Salvatore J, Anderies JM, Popescu G (2011) Modeling human ecodynamics and biocultural interactions in the Late Pleistocene of Western Eurasia. Hum Ecol 39:705–725

    Google Scholar 

  • Biondi IM, Bo MS, Vassallo AI (2010) Inter-individual and age differences in the exploration, neophilia and problem solving ability in a Neotropical raptor (Milvago chimango). Anim Cogn 13:701–710

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C (1995) Innovation in wild chimpanzees. Int J Primatol 16(1):1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C (2003) Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? Evol Anthropol 12:82–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek M, Heller S, Birch S, Henrich J (2012) Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evol Hum Behav 33(1):46–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole EF, Quinn JL (2012) Personality and problem solving performance explain competitive ability in the wild. Proc R Soc B 279:1168–1175

    Google Scholar 

  • Creanza N, Fogarty L, Feldman MW (2013) Exploring cultural Niche construction from the paleolithic to modern hunter-gatherers. In: Akazawa T et al (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol 1: cultural perspectives, replacement of Neanderthals by modern humans Series. doi:10.1007/978-4-431-545118_13

  • Csibra C, Gergely G (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn Sci 13(4):148–153. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005. Epub Mar 13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra C, Gergely G (2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:1149–1157

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean GL, Hoppitt W, Laland KN, Kendall RL (2011) Sex ratio affects sex-specific innovation and learning in captive ruffled lemurs (Varecia variegata and Varecia rubra). Am J Primatol 73:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBoer W (1990) Interaction, imitation, and communication as expressed in style: the Ucayali experience. In: Conkey M, Hastorf C (eds) The uses of style in archaeology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 82–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond J (1999) Guns, germs, and steel. W.W. Norton and Co, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis A (2005) Why we are social animals: the high road to imitation as social glue. In: Hurley S, Chater N (eds) Perspectives on imitation: from neuroscience to social science, vol. 2, imitation, human development, and culture. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 207–220

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Strimling P, Eriksson K, Laland K, Sjostrand J (2010) One cultural parent makes no culture. Anim Behav 79:1353–1362. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Ghirlanda S, Eriksson K (2011) Modelling the evolution and diversity of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:412–423. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogarty L, Strimling P, Leland KN (2011) The evolution of teaching. Evolution 65:2760–2770

    Google Scholar 

  • Galef B Jr (1992) The question of animal culture. Hum Nat 3:157–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Csibra G (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of human culture. In: Enfield NJ, Levinson SC (eds) Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition, and human interaction. Berg, Oxford, pp 229–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely B, Egyed K, Kiraly I (2007) On pedagogy. Dev Sci 10:139–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (2003) Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evol Hum Behav 24:153–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell M (2013) Complexity and the ten-thousand-hour rule. The New Yorker. Retrieved 10 Jun 2014 from http://www.newyorker.com/online/blogs/sportingscene/2013/08/psychology-ten-thousand-hour-rule-complexity.html

  • Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe. Belknap, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordan R (2005) Ethnologue, languages of the world, 15th edn, published in 1951. SIL International, Dallas

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosselain OP (1998) Social and technical identity in a clay crystal ball. In: Stark M (ed) The archaeology of social boundaries. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC, pp 78–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield PM (1984) A theory of the teacher in the learning activities of everyday life. In: Rogoff B, Lave J (eds) Everyday cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 117–138

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2001) Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral. Am Anthropol New Ser 103(4):992–1013

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. Am Antiq 69:197–221. doi:10.2307/4128416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2010) The evolution of innovation enhancing institutions. In: O’Brien M, Shennan SJ (eds) Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 99–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles SC, Fehr E, McElreath R, Barr A, Smith N, Henrich N, Hill K, Gil-White F, Gurven M, Marlowe F, Patton J, Tracer D (2005) ‘Economic man’ in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behav Brain Sci 28(6):795–855

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS (1992) Intimate fathers: the nature and context of Aka Pygmy paternal infant care. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BL (2012) Listen, here is a story: ethnographic life narratives from Aka and Ngandu women of the Congo Basin. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BL (2013) “Ekeloko” the spirit to create: innovation and social learning among Aka adolescents of the central African rainforest. In: Akazawa et al (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol 1: cultural perspectives, replacement of Neanderthals by modern humans series. doi:10.1007/978-4-431-545118_13

  • Hewlett BS, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. Am Anthropol 88:922–934

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, van de Koppel JMH, van de Koppel M (1986) Causes of death among Aka pygmies of the Central African Republic. In: Cavalli-Sforza LL (ed) African Pygmies. Academic, New York, pp 15–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Fouts H, Boyette A, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:1168–1178

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Dira S, Berl R (2013) Initial findings of Chabu demographic data (unpublished)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill K (2002) Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Hum Nat 13:105–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Inkeles A (2000) Measuring social capital and its consequences. Policy Sci 33:245–268

    Google Scholar 

  • Kameda T, Nakanishi D (2002) Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment. Evol Hum Behav 23:373–393

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan H, Hill K (1985) Food sharing among Ache foragers: tests of explanatory hypothesis. Curr Anthropol 26:223–245

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaneko, M (2014) Variations in shape, local classification, and establishment of a chaîne opératoire for pot-making among woman potters in Southwestern Ethiopia. Paper presented at RNMH workshop for Springer A02 book, Kyoto

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir G (1982) Relationships between social structure and response to novelty in captive jackdaws, Corvus monedula, response to novel space. Behavior 81:231–263

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleibeuker SW, Koolschijn PC MP, Jolles DD, De Dreu CKW, Crone EA (2013) The neural coding of creative idea generation across adolescence and early adulthood. Front Hum Neurosci 7:905. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kummer H, Goodall J (1985) Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 308:203–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Reader SM (1999) Foraging innovation in the guppy. Anim Behav 57:331–340

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalande K, Reader S (2010) Comparative perspectives on human innovation. In: O’Brien M, Shennan SJ (eds) Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 37–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre L, Reader SM, Sol D (2004) Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain Behav Evol 63:233–246. doi:10.1159/000076784

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann L, Aoki K, Feldman MW (2011) On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:424–435. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis J (2015) Where goods are free but knowledge costs: hunter-gatherer ritual economics in western central Africa. Hunter Gatherer Res 1(1):1–27 (Online ISSN: 1476-4261)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis HM, Laland KN (2012) Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. J R Anthropol Inst 367:2171–2180

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald DH, Hewlett BS (1999) Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Curr Anthropol 40:501–523

    Google Scholar 

  • Morand-Ferron J, Cole EF, Rawles JEC, Quinn JL (2011) Who are the innovators? A field experiment with 2 passerine species. Behav Ecol 22:1241–1248

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel J, Gue’rini C, Peze’ A, Rivet C (1999) The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In: Nadel J, Butterworth G (eds) Imitation in infancy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 209–234

    Google Scholar 

  • Nasir N (2005) Problem solving in technology-rich contexts: mathematics sense making in out-of-school environments. J Math Behav 24:275–286

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen M (2012) Imitation, pretend play and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture? J Comp Psychol 126:170–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen M, Bennett G, Subiaul F, Zentall T (2012) Introduction social learning in humans and nonhuman animals: theoretical and empirical dissections. J Comp Psychol 126(2):109–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida T (1987) Local traditions and cultural transmission. In: Smuts SS, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Strusaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 462–474

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien M, Shennan SJ (2010) Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Over H, Carpenter M (2012) Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. J Comp Psychol 126:182–192

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2012) Demography and variation in the accumulation of culturally inherited skills. In: O’Brien M, Shennan SJ (eds) Innovations in cultural systems contributions from evolutionary anthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 37–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader S, Laland K (2001) Primate innovation: sex, age and social rank differences. Int J Primatol 22(5):787–805

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader SM, Laland KN (2003) Animal innovation: an introduction. In: Reader SM, Laland KN (eds) Animal innovation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 3–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Roe P (1995) Style, society, myth, and structure. In: Carr C, Neitzel J (eds) Style, society and person: archaeological and ethnological perspectives. Plenum Press, New York, pp 27–76

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnoebelen T (2009) (Un)classifying the Shabu (unpublished source from author’s web site). Accessed 20 May 2014

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan S (2001) Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Camb Archaeol J 11(1):5–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan SJ, Steele J (1999) Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological approach. In: Box H, Gibson K (eds) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 367–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Spikins P, Hitchens G, Needham A, Rutherford H (2014) The cradle of thought: growth, learning, play and attachment in Neanderthal children. Oxf J Archaeol 33:111–134. doi:10.1111/ojoa.12030

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamnes CK, Ostby Y, Fjell AM, Westlye L, Due-Tonnessen P, Walhovd KB (2010) Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. Cereb Cortex 20:534–548

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Phil Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 364:2405–2415. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0052

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton A, Samson J (2012) Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats. Anim Behav 83:1459–1468

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1994) The question of chimpanzee culture. In: Wrangham R, McGrew W, de Waal F, Heltne P (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 301–317

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Kruger A, Ratner H (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:450–488

    Google Scholar 

  • van Bergen Y (2004) An Investigation into the adaptive use of social and asocial information. Dissertation, University of Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bonnie L. Hewlett .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Japan

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hewlett, B.L. (2016). Innovation, Processes of Social Learning, and Modes of Cultural Transmission Among the Chabu Adolescent Forager-Farmers of Ethiopia. In: Terashima, H., Hewlett, B.S. (eds) Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers. Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_17

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_17

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Tokyo

  • Print ISBN: 978-4-431-55995-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-4-431-55997-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics