Advertisement

Processing of the Global and Local Dimensions of Visual Hierarchical Stimuli by Humans (Homo sapiens), Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and Baboons (Papio papio)

  • Joël Fagot
  • Masaki Tomonaga
  • Christine Deruelle

Abstract

A well-known phenomenon in the literature on human perception is the global precedence effect, which was initially reported by Navon in 1977. Navon presented human subjects with hierarchical stimuli such as those shown in Fig. 1. These were large letters (global level) made up of smaller letters (local level) which had to be identified as quickly as possible. Use of these forms showed that response latencies were shorter on average for the global trials, involving identification of the global letter, than for the local trials, involving identification of the local letter. It was also shown that response times (RTs) in global trials remained unchanged whatever the identity of the letter shown at the local level, whereas RTs in local trials were higher when the global and local levels showed different letters than when they showed the same letter. On the basis of these results, (1977) suggested that the processing of visual stimuli by humans proceeds from an analysis of the global structure of the visual form prior to the analysis of its more local details. According to (1977), this global-to-local order of visual processing is a general trait of visual perception, and is independent of the use of hierarchical letter stimuli, as revealed by the title of his article “Forest before the tree: the precedence of global features in visual perception.”

Keywords

Display Size Visual Search Task Stimulus Level Precedence Effect Global Shape 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Basso MR, Schefft BK, Ris MD, Dember WN (1996) Mood and global-local visual processing. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 3:249–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Boer LC, Keuss PJG (1982) Global precedence as a postperceptual effect: an analysis of speed-accuracy tradeoff functions. Percept Psychophys 31:352–366Google Scholar
  3. Cerella J (1982) Mechanisms of concept formation in pigeon. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJ (eds) Analysis of visual behaviour. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  4. Cook RG, Cavato KK, Cavato, BR (1996) Mechanisms of multidimensional grouping, fusion, and search in avian texture discrimination. J Exp Psychol, Anim Behav Process 24:150–167Google Scholar
  5. Delis DC, Roberston LC, Efron R (1986) Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchical stimuli. Neuropsychologia 24:205–214PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Deruelle C, Fagot J (1998) Visual search for global/local stimulus features in humans and baboons. Psychonom Bull Rev 5:476–481Google Scholar
  7. De Valois RL, De Valois KK (1990) Spatial vision. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Fagot J, Deruelle C (1997) Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric specialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Performance 23:429–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fagot J, Tomonaga M (1999) Comparative assessment of global-local processing in humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): use of a visual search task with compound stimuli. J Comp Physiol 113:3–12Google Scholar
  10. Fagot J, Kruschke JK, Dépy D, Vauclair J (1998) Associativé learning in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio): species differences in learned attention to features. Anim Cogn 1:123–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Filoteo JV, Delis DC, Massman PJ, Demadura T (1992). Directed and divided attention in Alzheimer’s disease: impairment in shifting attention to global and local stimuli. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14:871–883PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fobes JL, King JE (1982) Vision: the dominant primate modality. In: Fobes JL, King JE (eds) Primate behavior. Academic Press, New York, pp 219–243Google Scholar
  13. Found A, Müller HJ (1997) Local and global orientation in visual search. Percept Psychophys 59:941–963PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Fremouw T, Walter TH, Shimp CP (1998) Priming of attention to local and global levels of visual analysis. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 24:278–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fujita K, Matsuzawa T (1990) Delayed figure reconstruction by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 104:345–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gibson EJ (1969) Principle of perceptual learning and development. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  17. Harnad S (1987) Categorical perception. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Hopkins WD (1997) Hemispheric specialization for local and global processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Neuropsychologia 35:343–348PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Horel JA (1994) Local and global perception examined by reversible suppression of temporal cortex with cold. Behav Brain Res 65:157–164PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kimchi R (1988) Selective attention to global and local levels in the comparison of hierarchical patterns. Percept Psychophys 43:189–198PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Kinchla RA, Wolfe JM (1979) The order of visual processing: “top down,” “bottom up,” or “middle-out”. Percept Psychophys 25:225–231PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Kinchla RA, Solis-Macias V, Hoffman J (1983) Attending to different levels of structure in the visual image. Percept Psychophys 33:1–10PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Koffka KA (1935) Principles of gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace & World, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  24. Martin M (1979) Local and global processing: the role of sparsity. Mem Cogn 7:476–484Google Scholar
  25. Matsuzawa T (1990) Form perception and visual acuity in a chimpanzee. Folia Primatol 55:24–32PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Navon D (1977) Forest before the tree: the precedence of global feature in visual perception. Cogn Psychol 9:353–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Navon D (1991) Testing a queue hypothesis for the processing of global and local information. J Exp Psychol: Gen 120:173–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Saarinen J (1994) Visual search for global and local stimulus features. Perception 23:237–243PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Treisman A, Gelade G (1980) A feature integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol 12:97–136PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ward LM (1982) Determinants of attention to local and global features of visual forms. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Performance 8:562–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Wilson JR, Lavallee KA, Joosse MV, Hendrickson AE, Boothe RG, Harwerth RS (1989) Visual field of monocularly deprived macaque monkeys. Behav Brain Res 33:13–22PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joël Fagot
    • 1
  • Masaki Tomonaga
    • 2
  • Christine Deruelle
    • 1
  1. 1.CNRSMarseille cedex 20France
  2. 2.Primate Research InstituteKyoto UniversityInuyama, AichiJapan

Personalised recommendations