Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Contributions to Management Science ((MANAGEMENT SC.))

  • 2187 Accesses

This final chapter is concerned with how performance appraisals should be conducted—i.e., with the appraisal process. Appraisal process models by cognitive psychologists typically distinguish three stages of the appraisal process: The collection of information for appraising someone, its organization and storage in memory, and its retrieval and integration into a coherent judgment for the respective appraisal purposes.1 Understanding the cognitive processes related to appraisals of performance helps design the appraisal system such that the purposes and goals of the appraisal can be achieved. Hence, the findings of cognitive psychologists will be referred to at several points of this chapter.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Ilgen et al. (1993), Bretz et al. (1992).

  2. 2.

    Longenecker et al. (1987), Ilgen (1993), Landy and Farr (1980).

  3. 3.

    Longenecker et al. (1987).

  4. 4.

    Longenecker et al. (1987), Foucault (1981), Cleveland and Murphy (1992), Jawahar and Williams (1997), Gomez-Mejia (1989), Fletcher (1997), cf. Sect. 5.5.

  5. 5.

    Longenecker et al. (1987, p. 183).

  6. 6.

    Longenecker and Gioia (1994).

  7. 7.

    Other examples of rater error and bias include halo, severity, central tendency, contrast, stereotype, similar-to-me, liking, friendship, first impression, and recency effects. The rater errors discussed most often in the literature are leniency, halo, and central tendency (Woehr and Hoffcutt 1994). Leniency refers to a tendency of appraisers to overrate their employees; halo refers to a tendency of providing exceptional ratings across all performance dimensions for someone who has performed exceptionally in one performance dimension that is particularly evident to the rater but was less effective in some other performance dimensions; and central tendency refers to a tendency to rate in categories in the middle of the rating scale, avoiding having to justify outstandingly good or bad ratings. Often, rater errors and accuracy are examined as separate outcome measures of the appraisal process, although, logically, rater errors should only matter in-so-far as they affect the accuracy of appraisal. Research on rater training (Bernardin and Pence 1980) showed that rater training that was aimed at reducing rater error not only reduced rater error but also rating accuracy. The training program that was examined involved definitions and distributional examples of halo, i.e., the training participants were told that high ratings equally distributed across different performance dimensions represented a rater error—halo. The study also measured halo on the basis of rating distributions across different dimensions. Halo and accuracy were measured before and after the training course. The fact that halo—as defined in the study—decreased together with rating accuracy indicates that what was defined and observed as halo did not actually represent halo but a tendency of outstanding performers to excel across different dimensions of performance. Actual halo would occur in a situation where someone actually performs exceptionally well in one performance factor, overshadowing deficiencies in other areas. This, however, is difficult to tell apart from the situation described above. Nevertheless, if it was observable, one would expect to observe a negative correlation between this rater error and rating accuracy.

  8. 8.

    E.g., Folger et al. (1992).

  9. 9.

    Schleicher and Day (1998).

  10. 10.

    Findley et al. (2000).

  11. 11.

    Folger et al. (1992).

  12. 12.

    Giles et al. (1997), Findley et al. (2000).

  13. 13.

    Bernardin et al. (1995), Murphy (1994).

  14. 14.

    Oechsler (2006). The German works council is an employee representative body, which must be elected in all plants with at least six employees. Cf. also Muller-Jentsch (1995).

  15. 15.

    Schiek (2004), EU Regulation 2000/43/EG.

  16. 16.

    Bretz et al. (1992) integrated the results of two major surveys of appraisal practices in 3,052 and 435 organizations and their own survey of appraisal practices of the Fortune Industrial 100.

  17. 17.

    Fletcher (1997, p.74).

  18. 18.

    Fletcher (1997, p.74).

  19. 19.

    Lohaus and Kleinmann (2002).

  20. 20.

    Lohaus and Kleinmann (2002).

  21. 21.

    Greguras et al. (2003).

  22. 22.

    Greguras et al. (2003).

  23. 23.

    Lohaus and Kleinmann (2002).

  24. 24.

    Fletcher (1997, p.77).

  25. 25.

    Bretz et al. (1992), Lohaus and Kleinmann (2002), Fletcher (1997).

  26. 26.

    Jawahar and Williams (1997).

  27. 27.

    Weighting more heavily does not necessarily imply numerical scaling factors. If that is not desired, rating dimensions may be aggregated into an overall rating in a qualitative fashion by the responsible supervisor, taking into account the assessments by some sources to a greater extent than those by other sources.

  28. 28.

    Cf. Sects. 3.1.2 and 4.1

  29. 29.

    Lohaus and Kleinman (2002).

  30. 30.

    Lohaus and Kleinman (2002).

  31. 31.

    Cf. Sect. 4.1 on the stability of general mental ability and personality traits of grown ups.

  32. 32.

    Cf. Sect. 4.1 on the stability of general mental ability and personality traits of grown ups.

  33. 33.

    E.g., Campbell et al. (1993), Ilgen et al. (1993), DeNisi (1997), Landy and Farr (1980).

  34. 34.

    Schleicher and Day (1998), Carlston (1994).

  35. 35.

    Schleicher and Day (1998).

  36. 36.

    Sulsky and Day (1992).

  37. 37.

    Cf. Sect. 5.6.

  38. 38.

    Fletcher (1997, p.20).

  39. 39.

    Jawahar and Williams (1997).

  40. 40.

    Jawahar and Williams (1997).

  41. 41.

    Cf. Sect. 4.2.

  42. 42.

    Smith and Kendall (1963).

  43. 43.

    Dunnette et al. (1968).

  44. 44.

    Wiersma and Latham (1986).

  45. 45.

    Cf. Ilgen et al. (1993).

  46. 46.

    Borman (1979, p. 419), also Fay and Latham (1982), Milkovich and Wigdor (1991).

  47. 47.

    Cf. Sect. 5.6.

  48. 48.

    Milkovich and Wigdor (1991). This finding is also supported by more fundamental research showing that people can cognitively process no more than seven (plus or minus two) items at a time (Miller 1956). Cf. also Drewes and Runde (2002), Fletcher (1997).

  49. 49.

    Bretz et al. (1992, p. 333).

  50. 50.

    Teel (1986).

  51. 51.

    Fletcher (1997), Bretz et al. (1992).

  52. 52.

    The argument is inspired by the work of Edward Deming (1986), who has provocatively labeled performance appraisal as one of seven deadly diseases of management. He suggests that the performance of individuals does not differ significantly and that variations are due to random observations, sampling error, factors outside the control of the individual, and the system in general. Any remaining differences in performance contributions by the individuals cannot be meaningfully differentiated from other factors by managers or other raters. There is partial empirical support for his argument, as, for instance, Greguras et al. (2003) find that “the combined rater and rater-by-ratee interaction effect and the residual effect were substantially larger than the person effect (the object of measurement)” (p.13). Earlier studies, by contrast, find that the largest source of variance in ratings is in fact the performance of the ratee (Landy and Farr 1980).

  53. 53.

    Cf. Sect. 3.1.

  54. 54.

    Zenger (1992).

  55. 55.

    Lawler (2003, p. 400).

  56. 56.

    Lawler (2003, p. 402).

  57. 57.

    Lawler (2003); Peck (1984).

  58. 58.

    It will be explained in Sect. 5.6 how abstract cognitive processing of raters can be supported through appraisal training.

  59. 59.

    Kluger and DeNisi (1996, p.255).

  60. 60.

    The psychological literature refers to factual feedback about performance results as knowledge of results (KR) interventions.

  61. 61.

    Thus, the concept of feedback interventions is somewhat narrower than that of feedback as it covers only targeted and deliberate provision of feedback. Consider, for example, a definition of feedback by London (1997, p.11): “Feedback is the information people receive about their performance. It conveys information about behaviors, and it conveys an evaluation about the quality of those behaviors.”

  62. 62.

    Kluger and DeNisi (1996). The theories they draw from include Thorndike's law of effect, control theory, goal setting theory, multiple-cue probability paradigm, social cognition theory, and learned helplessness theory.

  63. 63.

    Kluger and DeNisi (1996).

  64. 64.

    Kluger and DeNisi (1996).

  65. 65.

    Cf. London (1997).

  66. 66.

    Cf. Sect. 5.5.

  67. 67.

    Cf. London and Smither (2002).

  68. 68.

    Harris et al. (1995), for example, report on coaching as a rating dimension of assessment centers. Borman and Motowidlo (1993) identify “training, coaching, and developing subordinates” as one of six megadimensions in their competency framework for managers.

  69. 69.

    Hebert and Vorauer (2003).

  70. 70.

    Ford and Weldon (1981), Rozelle and Baxter (1981), Mero et al. (2003).

  71. 71.

    Hebert and Vorauer (2003); also Shore and Tashchian (2002), Klimoski and Inks (1990), Fisher (1979), Ilgen and Knowlton (1980); cf. Sect. 5.5.

  72. 72.

    The meta-analysis of the empirical literature by Kluger and DeNisi (1996) finds that, on average, verbal FIs “are likely” to attenuate FI effects. It is proposed that this effect would not have been observed if it had been possible to control for a variable such as “flexibility of the team structure.” The analysis does control for task complexity, which displays relatively low interjudge reliability, though, reflecting “the difficulty in conceptualizing task complexity” (p.275). Furthermore, task complexity is not equivalent to the flexibility of the team structure. Hence, due to the difficulty of measuring the contingency variable in question, the present work relies on the logic of the argument described above. The same applies to Kluger and DeNisi's empirical finding that some normative FIs (such as praise) generally impact negatively on performance.

  73. 73.

    Kluger and DeNisi (1996).

  74. 74.

    Firms such as Coca Cola Foods and PWC have been reported to practice coaching of managers by senior mentors (Seijts and Latham 2005). Cf. Sect. 5.5.

  75. 75.

    Longenecker et al. (1987, p. 184).

  76. 76.

    Longenecker et al. (1987, p. 184), Cleveland and Murphy (1992).

  77. 77.

    Longenecker et al. (1987, p. 184), Cleveland and Murphy (1992).

  78. 78.

    London (1997, p. 152).

  79. 79.

    McAllister et al. (1979), Ford and Weldon (1981), Tetlock (1983a, 1983b; 1985a, 1985b).

  80. 80.

    Longenecker et al. (1987, p. 186).

  81. 81.

    Frink and Ferris (1998).

  82. 82.

    E.g., Ford and Weldon (1981), Rozelle and Baxter (1981), Mero et al. (2003), Walker and Smither (1999), Larson (1984).

  83. 83.

    Shore and Tashchian (2002), Klimoski and Inks (1990), Fisher (1979), Ilgen and Knowlton (1980).

  84. 84.

    Tetlock (1983a).

  85. 85.

    Klimoski and Inks (1990, p.197), cf. Shore et al. (1988).

  86. 86.

    Klimoski and Inks (1990).

  87. 87.

    See below paragraphs on heuristics.

  88. 88.

    Cf. Tetlock (1985c; 1992), Tetlock and Boettger (1989), Tetlock and Kim (1987), Tetlock et al. (1989).

  89. 89.

    The academic literature distinguishes between distributive and procedural justice. Distributive justice concerns the equitability of a received outcome, i.e., the ratio of someone's own outcomes (e.g., performance ratings or pay) to inputs (e.g., performance levels) compared to the ratio of other people. Procedural justice, by contrast, is concerned with the fairness of the processes that led to a particular outcome. According to a two-component model of justice (Cropanzano and Folger 1996), distributive injustice energizes behavior, i.e., motivates people to do something about it. The direction of this behavior is determined by the perceived degree of procedural justice. If procedures are perceived to be fair, employees will respond with constructive behavior, expecting that this behavior will lead to the desired outcomes in future. If, however, procedures are perceived to be unfair, employees are more likely to retaliate through destructive behavior, expecting that constructive responses are unlikely to lead to the desired outcomes anyway. The conclusion that increased appraisal fairness results in positive motivational effects among appraisees can also be arrived at using expectancy theory (Vroom 1964). This theory suggests that motivation is dependent on the attractiveness of anticipated consequences of performance for the individual (“valence”), the perceived probability that effort will lead to performance on a task or job (“expectancy”), and the individual's confidence that a particular level of performance will lead to a particular level of outcomes (“instrumentality”). Thus, increased fairness of the performance appraisal can be seen to have a positive impact on instrumentality, thus, increasing the motivation of the appraisees. Schleicher and Day (1998) provide empirical evidence for a positive impact on employee attitudes of appraisal procedures that are perceived to be fair.

  90. 90.

    Landy et al. (1978).

  91. 91.

    Kandel and Frumer (1994, p. 587).

  92. 92.

    Rowe (1991, p.356).

  93. 93.

    A broader definition is adopted by Rowe (1991) who describes “an internal ombudsman as a neutral or impartial manager within an organization, who may provide informal and confidential assistance to managers and employees in resolving work-related concerns; who may serve as a counselor, informal go-between and facilitator, formal mediator, informal fact-finder, upward-feedback mechanism, consultant, problem prevention device and change agent; and whose office is located outside ordinary line management structures” (p.353).

  94. 94.

    Rowe (1991, p. 359).

  95. 95.

    Rowe (1991, p. 356).

  96. 96.

    Kandel and Frumer (1994, p.587).

  97. 97.

    Landy et al. (1978), cf. above.

  98. 98.

    Professional associations include the International Ombudsman Association (www.ombuds-toa. org) and the Ombudsman Association (http://web.mit.edu/negotiation/toa). Usually, Ombudspeople are employed as regular employees, but they may also be hired as contractors to emphasize their independence from formal and informal organizational structures (Kandel and Frumer 1994, p. 591). Also cf. Fernie and Metcalf (2004).

  99. 99.

    Elsewhere the mentor is defined as an influential individual, with advanced experience and knowledge, who is committed to providing upward support and mobility to his/her protégé's career (Levinson et al. 1978). Kram (1995) assigns two broad functions to mentoring—career development and psychological support. The former involves vocational support, including coaching, advising, exposing the protégé to key players in the organization, providing technical support and advice on specific skill development, minimizing protégé involvement in situations that may be political or controversial, and nominating the protégé for promotion. As part of the psychological support function, the mentor serves as a confidant, enhances the protégé's sense of competence, self-efficacy, and professional and personal development, and serves as a role model providing inspiration to the protégé (Scandura and Hamilton 2002).

  100. 100.

    Clawson and Newburg (2002, p. 311).

  101. 101.

    Clawson and Newburg (2002, p. 311).

  102. 102.

    Clawson and Newburg (2002, p. 314) also cf. Scandura and Hamilton (2002).

  103. 103.

    Woehr and Huffcutt (1994), see below.

  104. 104.

    Bretz et al. (1992), Fletcher (1997), Drewes and Runde (2002). It has been shown that the positive effects of appraisal trainings begin to wear off significantly 6 months after the training, suggesting that they should be conducted on a more regular basis (Ivancevich 1979; Bernardin 1978).

  105. 105.

    Fletcher (1997, p. 83).

  106. 106.

    Cf. Ilgen (1993), Landy and Farr (1980), Longenecker et al. (1987).

  107. 107.

    Campbell (1989).

  108. 108.

    The term was first used in a publication by Bernardin and Buckley in 1981. Cf. Sulsky and Day (1992; 1994); also Woehr and Huffcutt (1994).

  109. 109.

    Cf. Chap.4 on personal construct theory and Sect. 5.3 on associated systems theory.

  110. 110.

    Pulakos (1984; 1986), Noonan and Sulsky (2001), Fletcher (1997).

  111. 111.

    Schleicher and Day (1998), Sulsky and Day (1992).

  112. 112.

    Noonan and Sulsky (2001).

  113. 113.

    Noonan and Sulsky (2001, p.6).

  114. 114.

    That is, letting the situation or setting influence observations.

  115. 115.

    That is, forcing observations into categories instead of remembering the differences between ideas, behavior, and people.

  116. 116.

    Campbell (1958), Latham et al. (1975), Thornton and Zorich (1980), Hedge and Kavanagh (1988).

  117. 117.

    Thornton and Zorich (1980).

  118. 118.

    Pulakos (1986), Hedge and Kavanagh (1988), Bernardin and Walter (1977), Thornton and Zorich (1980), Latham et al. (1975).

  119. 119.

    Observational accuracy refers to a rater's ability to recall specific behavioral events.

  120. 120.

    Woehr and Huffcutt (1994, p.200).

  121. 121.

    Noonan and Sulsky (2001), also Smith (1986).

  122. 122.

    Pulakos (1986).

  123. 123.

    Bernardin and Pence (1980), Bernardin and Buckley (1981), Latham (1986).

  124. 124.

    Levy and Williams (2004).

  125. 125.

    Noonan and Sulsky (2001).

  126. 126.

    Schleicher and Day (1998) find that due process appraisal systems are characterized by adequate notice, fair hearing, and judgments based on evidence.

  127. 127.

    Cf. Fletcher (1997), Drewes and Runde (2002).

  128. 128.

    London (1997).

  129. 129.

    Fletcher (1997), Drewes and Runde (2002).

  130. 130.

    A review of 24 studies of appraisal trainings (Smith 1986) found that presenting training material on the basis of a lecture alone was mostly ineffective in improving rating accuracy. Despite this, lectures were found to be the most commonly used training method, on the grounds that they are the least time consuming. Methods that include one or more practical exercises followed by feedback from the trainer were found to be mostly effective in improving rating accuracy. Group discussions in combination with practical exercises further enhanced accuracy.

  131. 131.

    Cf. Hebert and Vorauer (2003).

  132. 132.

    Fletcher (1997, p. 46).

  133. 133.

    Fletcher (1997, p. 46).

  134. 134.

    Longenecker et al. (1987), cf. Sect. 5.5.

  135. 135.

    Generally, it has been suggested that training sessions should last for at least 3 h to be effective (Smith 1986; Noonan and Sulsky 2001; Fay and Latham 1982). Rating and feedback giving behaviors are well-ingrained habits, which are difficult to eliminate. Only repeated rehearsals of the new behavioral scripts ensure that they are also adopted during the actual appraisal process. Depending on the amount of content to be covered during the training, the duration of the training can also be longer. Some of the training sessions reviewed lasted up to 14 h (Smith 1986). Busy managers at the top- and mid-levels of the organizational hierarchy may have difficulties to find an uninterrupted time slot for a training course lasting for several hours. It has been suggested to break down the training for them into a couple of separate sessions (Fletcher 1997).

  136. 136.

    Fletcher (1997, p. 98).

  137. 137.

    Fletcher (1997, p. 98).

  138. 138.

    Bretz et al. (1992).

  139. 139.

    Korsgaard and Roberson (1995).

  140. 140.

    Fletcher (1997).

  141. 141.

    Fletcher (1997).

  142. 142.

    Ivancevic (1979), Bernardin (1978).

  143. 143.

    Cf. subsection “slicing the data,” Sect. 4.3.5.

References

  • Bernardin HJ (1978) Effects of rater training on leniency and halo errors in student ratings of instructors. J Appl Psychol 63:301–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernardin HJ, Buckley MR (1981) Strategies in rater training. Acad Manage Rev 6:205–212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernardin HJ, Pence EC (1980) Effects of rater training: new response sets and decreasing accuracy. J Appl Psychol 65:60–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernardin HJ, Walter CS (1977) Effects of rater training and diary-keeping on psychometric error in ratings. J Appl Psychol 62:64–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernardin HJ, Kane JS, Ross S, Spina JD, Johnson DL (1995) Performance appraisal design, development, and implementation. In: Ferris RG, Rosen SD, Barnum DT (eds) Handbook of human resource management. Cambridge, MA, Blackwell, pp 462–493

    Google Scholar 

  • Borman WC, Motowidlo SJ (1993) Expanding the criterion domain to include elements of contextual performance. In: Schmitt N, Borman WC (eds) Personnel selection in organizations. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp 71–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretz RD Jr, Milkovich GT, Read W (1992) The current state of performance appraisal research and practice: concerns, directions, and implications. J Manage 18(2):321–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell DT (1958) Systematic error on the part of human links in communication systems. Inf Control 1:334–369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell JP (1989) The agenda for theory and research. In: Goldstein IL et al (eds) Training and development in organizations. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, pp 469–486

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell JP, McCloy RA, Oppler SH, Sager CE (1993) A theory of performance. In: Neal Schmitt, Walter CB (eds) Personnel selection in organizations. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp 35–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlston DE (1994) Associated systems theory: a systematic approach to cognitive representations of persons. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Clawson JG, Newburg DS (2002) Mentoring for world-class performance. In: Sonnentag S (ed) Psychological management of individual performance. Wiley, Chichester, West Sussex, UK, pp 309–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleveland JN, Murphy KR (1992) Analyzing performance appraisal as goal-directed behavior. Res Pers Hum Resour Manage 10:121–185

    Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano R, Folger R (1996) Procedural justice and worker motivation. In: Steers RM, Porter LW, Bigley GA (eds) Motivation and leadership at work, 6th edn. McGraw-Hill, New York, pp 72–83

    Google Scholar 

  • DeNisi AS (1997) Cognitive approach to performance appraisal: a programme of research. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Drewes G, Runde B (2002) Performance appraisal. In: Sonnentag S (ed) Psychological management of individual performance. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 137–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunnette MD, Campbell JP, Hellervik LW (1968) Job behavior scales for Penney Co. department managers. Personnel Decisions, Minneapolis

    Google Scholar 

  • Fay CH, Latham GP (1982) Effects of training and rating scales on rating errors. Pers Psychol 35:105–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernie S, Metcalf D (2004) The organisational ombuds: implications for voice, conflict resolution and fairness at work. In: Kaufman B (ed) Advances in Industrial and Labour Relations, vol 13. JAI, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Findley HM, Mossholder KW, Giles WF (2000) Performance appraisal process and system facets: relationships with contextual performance. J Appl Psychol 85(4):634–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher CD (1979) Transmission of positive and negative feedback to subordinates: a laboratory investigation. J Appl Psychol 64:533–540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher C (1997) Appraisal–routes to improved performance, 3rd edn. Institute of Personnel and Development, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Folger R, Konovsky MA, Cropanzano R (1992) Due process metaphor for performance appraisal. Res Organ Behav 14:129–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford JK, Weldon E (1981) Forewarning and accountability: effects on memory-based interpersonal judgments. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 7:264–268

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault M (1981) Power/knowledge: selected interviews and other writings. Harvester, Brighton

    Google Scholar 

  • Frink DD, Ferris GR (1998) Accountability, impression management, and goal setting in the performance evaluation process. Hum Relat 51(10):1259–1283

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles WF, Findley HM, Field HS (1997) Procedural fairness in performance appraisal: beyond the review session. J Bus Psychol 11:493–506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez-Mejia LR (1989) Performance appraisal: testing a process model. J Manag Psychol 4(3):27–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Greguras GJ, Robie C, Schleicher DJ, Goff M III (2003) A field study of the effects of rating purpose on the quality of multisource ratings. Pers Psychol 56:1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris MM, Smith DE, Champagne D (1995) A field study of performance appraisal purpose: research vs administrative-based ratings. Pers Psychol 48(1):151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebert BG, Vorauer JD (2003) Seeing through the screen: is evaluative feedback communicated more effectively in face-to-face or computer-mediated exchanges. Comput Human Behav 19:25–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedge JW, Kavanagh MJ (1988) Improving the accuracy of performance evaluations: comparison of three methods of performance appraiser training. J Appl Psychol 73:68–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ilgen DR (1993) Performance-appraisal accuracy: an illusive or sometimes misguided goal? In: Schuler H, Farr JL, Smith M (eds) Personnel selection and assessment: individual and organizational perspectives. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 235–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Ilgen DR, Barnes-Farrell JL, McKellin DB (1993) Performance appraisal process research in the 1980s: what has it contributed to performance appraisal in use? Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 54:321–368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ilgen DR, Knowlton WA (1980) Performance attributional effects on feedback from supervisors. Organ Behav Hum Perform 25:441–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ivancevich JM (1979) Longitudinal study of the effects of rater training on psychometric error in ratings. J Appl Psychol 64:502–508

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jawahar IM, Williams CR (1997) Where all the children are above average: the performance appraisal purpose effect. Pers Psychol 50(4):905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klimoski R, Inks L (1990) Accountability forces in performance appraisal. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 45(2):194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korsgaard MA, Roberson L (1995) Procedural justice in performance evaluation: the role of instrumental and non-instrumental voice in performance appraisal decisions. J Manage 21:657–669

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landy FJ, Farr JL (1980) Performance rating. Psychol Bull 87:72–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landy FJ, Barnes JL, Murphy KR (1978) Correlates of perceived fairness and accuracy of performance evaluation. J Appl Psychol 63:751–754

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larson JR (1984) The performance feedback process: a preliminary model. Organ Behav Hum Perform 33:42–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latham GP (1986) Job performance and appraisal. In: Cooper CL, Robertson I (eds) International review of industrial and organizational psychology. Wiley, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Latham GP, Wexley KN, Pursell FD (1975) Training managers to minimize rating errors in the observation of behavior. J Appl Psychol 60:550–555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler EE III (2003) Reward practices and performance management system effectiveness. Organ Dyn 32(4):396–404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson DJ, Darrow CN, Klein EB, Levinson MA, McKee B (1978) Seasons of a man's life. Knopf, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy PE, Williams JR (2004) The social context of performance appraisal: a review and framework for the future. J Manage 30(6):881–905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lohaus D, Kleinmann M (2002) Analysis of performance potential. In: Sonnentag S (ed) Psychological management of individual performance. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 155–178

    Google Scholar 

  • London M (1997) Job feedback. Giving, seeking, and using feedback for performance improvement. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • London M, Smither JW (2002) Feedback orientation, feedback culture, and the longitudinal performance management process. Hum Resour Manage Rev 12:81–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longenecker CO, Sims HP, Gioia DA (1987) Behind the mask: the politics of employee appraisal. Acad Manage Exec 1(3):183–193

    Google Scholar 

  • McAllister DW, Mitchell TR, Beach LR (1979) The contingency model for the selection of decision strategies: An empirical test of the effects of significance, accountability, and reversibility. Organ Behav Hum Perform 24:228–244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mero NP, Motowidlo SJ, Anna AL (2003) Effects of accountability on rating behavior and rater accuracy. J Appl Soc Psychol 33(12):2493–2514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milkovich GT, Wigdor AK (1991) Pay and performance: evaluating performance appraisal and merit pay. National Academy, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GA (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for possessing information. Psychol Rev 63:81–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller-Jentsch W (1995) Germany: from collective voice to co-management. In: Rogers J, Streeck W (eds) Works council. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 53–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Noonan LE, Sulsky LM (2001) Impact of frame-of-reference and behavioral observation training on alternative training effectiveness criteria in a Canadian military sample. Hum Perf 14(1):3–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oechsler WA (2006) Personal und Arbeit. Grundlagen des human resource management und der Arbeitgeber-Arbeitnehmer-Beziehungen, 8th edn. Oldenbourg, Munich, Germany

    Google Scholar 

  • Peck CA (1984) Pay and performance: the interaction of compensation and performance appraisal. Research bulletin no. 155. Conference Board, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulakos ED (1984) A comparison of rater training programs: error training and accuracy training. J Appl Psychol 69(4):581–588

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulakos ED (1986) The development of training programs to increase accuracy with different rating tasks. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 38:76–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scandura TA, Hamilton BA (2002) Enhancing performance through mentoring. In: Sonnentag S (ed) Psychological management of individual performance. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 293–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiek D (2004) Gleichbehandlungsrichtlinien der EU—Umsetzung im deutschen Arbeitsrecht, NZA. Heft 16:873–885

    Google Scholar 

  • Schleicher DJ, Day DV (1998) A cognitive evaluation of frame-of-reference rater training: content and process issues. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 73(1):76–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seijts GH, Latham GP (2005) Learning vs. performance goals: when should each be used. Acad Manag Exec 19:124–131

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore TH, Tashchian A (2002) Accountability forces in performance appraisal: effects of self-appraisal information, normative information, and task performance. J Bus Psychol 17(2):261–274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith DE (1986) Training programs for performance appraisal: a review. Acad Manage Rev 11(1):22–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith PC, Kendall LM (1963) Retranslation of expectations. J Appl Psychol 47:149–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sulsky LM, Day DV (1992) Frame-of-reference training and cognitive categorization: an empirical investigation of rater memory issues. J Appl Psychol 77:501–510

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teel KS (1986) Compensation: are merit raises really based on merit. Pers J, March, 65:88–95

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE (1983a) Accountability and complexity of thought. J Pers Soc Psychol 45:74–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE (1985c) Accountability: the neglected social context of judgment and choice. In: Cummings L, Staw BM (eds) Research in organizational behavior, vol 7. JAI, Greenwich, CT, pp 297–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE, Boettger R (1989) Accountability: social magnifier of the dilution effect. J Pers Soc Psychol 57:388–398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE, Kim JI (1987) Accountability and judgment processes in a personality prediction task. J Pers Soc Psychol 52:700–709

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE, Skitka L, Boettger R (1989) Social and cognitive strategies for coping with accountability: conformity, complexity, and bolstering. J Pers Soc Psychol 57:632–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton GC III, Zorich S (1980) Training to improve observer accuracy. J Appl Psychol 65:351–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vroom VH (1964) Work and motivation. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker AG, Smither JW (1999) A five-year study of upward feedback: what managers do with their results matters. Pers Psychol 52(2):393–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woehr DJ, Huffcutt AI (1994) Rater training for performance appraisal: a quantitative review. J Occup Organ Psychol 67:189–205

    Google Scholar 

  • Zenger TR (1992) Why do employers only reward extreme performance? Examining the relationships among performance, pay, and turnover. Admin Sci Quart 37:198–219

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

(2009). Appraisal Process. In: Krausert, A. (eds) Performance Management for Different Employee Groups. Contributions to Management Science. Physica-Verlag HD. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-7908-2197-0_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics