On the Other Side of the Moon: The Import of Implicit Knowledge for Gestalt Therapy

  • Daniel N. Stern
  • Boston Change Process Study Group


Thank you, Margherita and Giovanni. Actually, I guess this is our third meeting. We seem to have met through the literature before Palermo,’ which was a sort of preparing the territory. When we met in Palermo, I think that the three of us fell in love with each other. So the real reason why I’m here is to continue our affair. In fact, like most lovers, we’re going to have many disagreements, and that will be the fun part to talk about, I think.


Explicit Knowledge Emergent Property Present Moment Mirror Neuron Implicit Knowledge 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Braten S (1998) Infant learning by altero-centric participation: The reverse of egocentric observation in autism. In: Braten A (ed) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny. Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, pp. 105–124Google Scholar
  2. Damasio A (1994) Decartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. Putnam, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Husserl E (1960) Cartesian meditations (transi. Dorian Cairns). Martinus Nijohff, The HagueGoogle Scholar
  4. Husserl E (1989) Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. Second Book (transi. R. Rojcewicz & A. Schuwer). Kluwer Academic Publishers, DordrechtCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Meltzoff AN (1995) Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by eighteen-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 3: 838–850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Meltzoff AN, Gopnik A (1993) The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (eds) Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford Univ Press, New York, pp 335–366Google Scholar
  7. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 75–78PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Merleau-Ponty M (1945) Phénoménologie de la perception. Librairie Gallimard, Paris Mitchell SA (2000) Relationality: From attachment to intersubjectivity. Analytic Press, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  9. Salonia G (1992) Time and relation. Studies in Gestalt Therapy 1: 7–19Google Scholar
  10. Spagnuolo Lobb M (1992) Specific support for each interruption of contact. Studies in Gestalt Therapy 1: 43–51Google Scholar
  11. Spagnuolo Lobb M (2001) The theory of self in Gestalt therapy. A restatement of some aspects. Gestalt Review 4: 276–288Google Scholar
  12. Trevarthen C (1980) The foundation of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. In: Olson D (ed) The social foundation of language and thought. Norton, New York, pp 316–342Google Scholar
  13. Trevarthen C, Hubley P (1978) Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Wien 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Daniel N. Stern
  • Boston Change Process Study Group

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations