Skip to main content

Ziele, Volition und Handlungskontrolle

  • Chapter
  • 89k Accesses

Part of the book series: Springer-Lehrbuch ((SLB))

Zusammenfassung

Ziele geben dem Leben Sinn und Struktur und sind damit eine wesentliche Facette der persönlichen Identität. Was Menschen denken, tun und fühlen, wird von ihren Zielen bestimmt. Die Bedeutsamkeit von Zielen zeigt sich in einer Vielzahl an Theorien zu den Determinanten und Prozessen der Zielsetzung, Zielverfolgung und Zielablösung. So geht es einerseits um die Fragen, welcher Art Ziele Wohlbefinden und Leistung fördern, und wie man sich verbindliche Ziele setzen kann, die attraktiv und realistisch sind. Andererseits wird thematisiert, welche volitionalen Strategien einer Person zur Verfügung stehen, um die ausdauernde Verfolgung ihrer Ziele sicherzustellen. Dies verlangt nicht selten Selbstdisziplin und Willenskraft, was bisweilen als anstrengend erlebt wird. Doch nicht nur die Ausdauer ist eine wichtige Voraussetzung für erfolgreiches Zielstreben, sondern auch die Fähigkeit, von einem Ziel abzulassen, wenn es sich als unerreichbar, unattraktiv oder zu kostenreich erweist.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   29.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   37.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Literatur

  • Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2018). Motivation und Volition im Handlungsverlauf. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation und Handeln (5. Aufl., S. 355–388). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 93-130). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2010). Motivation. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 268-316). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: Stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 789–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. (2009). Responding to subliminal cues: Do if-then plans facilitate action preparation and initiation without conscious intent? Social Cognition, 27, 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention.Child Development, 78, 246–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. (2007). Das flexible Selbst: Selbstentwicklung zwischen Zielbindung und Ablösung. München: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2002). The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal adjustment: A two-process framework. Developmental Review, 22, 117–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., & Frank, E. (2002). Effects of deliberative and implemental mindsets on persistence in goal-directed behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1366–1378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., & Herrmann, M. (2017). Goal disengagement and action crises. In N. Baumann, T. Goschke, M. Kazén, S. Koole & M. Quirin (Eds.), Why people do the things they do: Building on Julius Kuhl‘s contribution to motivation and volition psychology (pp. 87–108). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., Herrmann, M., & Schüler, J. (2013). The struggle of giving up personal goals: Affective, physiological, and cognitive consequences of an action crisis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1668–1682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., & Schüler, J. (2013). Action crisis and cost–benefit thinking: A cognitive analysis of a goal-disengagement phase. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 543–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2002). Das Streben nach persönlichen Zielen: Emotionales Wohlbefinden und proaktive Entwicklung über die Lebensspanne. In G. H. J. Thomae & G. Jüttemann (Hrsg.), Persönlichkeit und Entwicklung (S. 157–190). Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2014). Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control. Has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Friedman, R. (2007). Approach-avoidance: A central characteristic of personal goals. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, action, and human flourishing (pp. 97–118). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 220–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Mind-sets and pre-conscious open-mindedness to incidental information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 48–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 303–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Wright, R. A., & Richter, M. (2012). Effort intensity: Some insights from the cardiovascular system. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of motivation (pp. 420–438). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S., Gollwitzer, P. M., Cohen, A.-L., Oettingen, G., & Burgess, P. W. (2009). Separable brain systems supporting cued versus self-initiated realization of delayed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 905–915.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Mindset theory of action phases. In P. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (vol. 1, pp. 526–545). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Deliberative vs. implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1119–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on the illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 531–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Planning promotes goal striving. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (3rd ed., pp. 223–244). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2008). Volition und kognitive Kontrolle. In J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Allgemeine Psychologie (S. 232–293). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). The representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211–1226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosse Holtforth, M., Grawe, K., & Castonguay, L. G. (2006). Predicting a reduction of avoidance motivation in psychotherapy: Toward the delineation of differential processes of change operating at different phases of treatment. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 639–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 1015–1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2018). Entwicklung der Motivation. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation und Handeln (5. Aufl., S. 493–540). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Dweck, C. S. (Hrsg.). (1998).Motivation and self-regulation across the life span. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, M., & Brandstätter, V. (2015). Action crises and goal disengagement: Longitudinal evidence on the predictive validity of a motivational phase in goal striving. Motivation Science, 1, 121–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heslin, P. A., Vandewalle, D., & Latham, G. P. (2006). Keen to help?: Mangers’ implicit person theories and their subsequent employee coaching. Personnel Psychology, 59, 871–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 450–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion – is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people‘s lives. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotabe, H. P., & Hofmann, W. (2015). On integrating the components of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 618–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal-systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state versus action orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983).Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle. Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1984). Motivational aspects of achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action control. In B. A. Maher & W. B. Maher (Eds.),Progress in experimental personality research (vol. 13, pp. 99–171). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1994). Action and state orientation: Psychometric properties of the action control scales (ACS-90). In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 47–59). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Interaktionen psychischer Systeme. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2018). Individuelle Unterschiede in der Selbststeuerung. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation und Handeln (5. Aufl., S. 389–422). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994). State orientation and the activation and retrieval of intentions from memory. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 127–154). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1994). Self-discrimination and memory: State orientation and false self-ascription of assigned activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1103–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Baldes, J. J. (1975). The »practical significance« of Locke‘s theory of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 122–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1926). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affekt-Psychologie II: Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 330–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (Eds.). (2013). New developments in goal setting and task performance. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of will-power. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. E., & Wrosch, C. (2007). You‘ve gotta know when to fold ‘em: Goal disengagement and systemic inflammation in adolescence. Psychological Science, 18, 773–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 249–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 83–105). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morsella, E., Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (Eds.). (2009). Oxfordhandbook of human action. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, G. B., & Grant, H. (Eds.). (2009). The psychology of goals. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G. (2012). Future thought and behavior change. European Review of Social Psychology, 23, 1–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Strategies of setting and implementing goals: Mental contrasting and implementation intentions. In J. E. Maddux & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Social psychological foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 114–135). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., & Reininger, K. M. (2016). The power of prospection: Mental contrasting and behavior change. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 591–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pervin, L. A. (1989). Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rattan, A., Savani, K., Chugh, D., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Leveraging mindsets to promote academic achievements: Policy recommendations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 721–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, M., Friedrich, A., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Task difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology, 45, 869–875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenstiel, L. von, Regnet, E., & Domsch, M. (Hrsg.). (2009). Führung von Mitarbeitern. Stuttgart: Schäffer-Poeschel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Jones, N. M., Davis, A. Q., & Kley, C. (2008). The role of implicit motivation in hot and cold goal pursuit: Effects on goal progress, goal rumination, and emotional well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 971–987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzer, R., Lippke, S., & Ziegelmann, J. P. (2008). Health action process approach: A research agenda at the Freie Universität Berlin to examine and promote health behavior change. Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie, 16,157–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, J. Y., & Gardner, W. L. (2008). Handbook of motivation science. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staw, B. M. (1997). The escalation of commitment: An update and appraisal. In Z. Shapira (Ed.), Organizational decision making (pp. 191-215). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affects help improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 379–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.). (2016). Handbook of self-regulation. Resarch, theory, and applications. New York, NY: Guildford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1929). Behaviorism – the modern note in psychology. In J. B. Wantson & W. McDougall (eds.), The battle of behaviorism (pp. 7–39). New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Schulz, R. (2003). The importance of goal disengagement in adaptive self-regulation: When giving up is beneficial. Self and Identity, 2, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., & Miller, G. E. (2013). Goal adjustment capacities, subjective well-being, and physical health. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 847–860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1494–1508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yaeger, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47, 302–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer-Verlag GmbH Deutschland, ein Teil von Springer Nature

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brandstätter, V., Schüler, J., Puca, R.M., Lozo, L. (2018). Ziele, Volition und Handlungskontrolle. In: Motivation und Emotion. Springer-Lehrbuch. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-56685-5_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-56685-5_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-56684-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-56685-5

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics