Skip to main content

Denken und Fühlen

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 28k Accesses

Zusammenfassung

In diesem Kapitel wird dargestellt, wie sich unser affektives Erleben und unser Denken (z. B. Informationsverarbeitung und Urteilsbildung) wechselseitig beeinflussen. Zunächst wird beschrieben, wie sich der Informationsverarbeitungsstil in Abhängigkeit von der jeweiligen Stimmung unterscheiden kann: Zum einen werden bei Wahrnehmung und Erinnerung stimmungskongruente Informationen besser verarbeitet. Darüber hinaus können von der Stimmung gefärbte Urteile auch entstehen, indem die Stimmung selbst als Information über den Urteilsgegenstand herangezogen wird. Des Weiteren wird in dem Kapitel dargelegt, welchen Einfluss Kognitionen auf die Entstehung und die Veränderung unseres affektiven Erlebens haben. So hängt das Entstehen spezifischer Emotionen stark von der Bewertung der emotionsauslösenden Situation ab. Darüber hinaus können weitgehend ohne die Beteiligung von Kognitionen Affekte durch Wahrnehmung und Imitation ausgelöst werden.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

eBook
USD   24.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   34.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Die Dauer von Emotionen kann ebenfalls – in Abhängigkeit von dem emotionsauslösenden Ereignis (z. B. kurze vs. lange Dauer des Ereignisses), Merkmale der Emotion (z. B. Intensität der Emotion) und Merkmale der emotionserlebenden Person (z. B. Unterschiede in der Regulation von Emotionen) – stark variieren (für einen Überblick vgl. Verduyn et al. 2015). Allgemein jedoch dauern Stimmungen im Vergleich zu Emotionen länger an.

  2. 2.

    Personen in positiver Stimmung benutzen im Vergleich zu schlecht gestimmten Personen häufig weniger aufwendige Informationsverarbeitungsstrategien, was aber weder auf eine eingeschränkte kognitive Kapazität noch auf eine geringere Motivation zurückgeführt werden kann. Vielmehr scheint es so, dass der Rückgriff auf übergeordnete Wissensstrukturen in positiver Stimmung eine andere, möglicherweise aufwendigere Verarbeitung in vielen Fällen unnötig macht (Bless et al. 1996a; Bless und Fiedler 2006; Bless et al. 1996b; Cote 2005; Isbell 2004; Krauth-Gruber und Ric 2000).

  3. 3.

    Neuere Forschung deutet daraufhin, dass nicht die jeweilige Stimmung einen entsprechenden Fokus induziert, sondern vielmehr bei positiver Stimmung der momentane Fokus beibehalten wird und bei negativer Stimmung ein Wechsel des Fokusses stattfindet. Da in der Regel ein globaler Fokus der momentane Fokus ist (der sog. Global-Precedence-Effekt ; z. B. Navon 1977), führt eine positive Stimmung zur Beibehaltung des Fokus und eine schlechte Stimmung zur Veränderung des globalen Fokus hin zu einem lokalen Fokus (für einen Überblick vgl. Huntsinger et al. 2014). Dies kann auch erklären, weswegen manche positive Stimmungen, die eine starke Annäherungsmotivation beinhalten (z. B. Verlangen), mit einem lokalen Fokus einhergehen (Gable und Harmon-Jones 2008; repliziert von Domachowska et al. 2016), da durch diese positive Stimmungen ein Ziel aktiviert wird, welches wiederum einem lokalen Fokus nach sich zieht (Huntsinger 2013).

  4. 4.

    Neben diesem generellen Effekt, dass positive Stimmung Kreativität fördert, gibt es eine Reihe von Moderatoren, die einen Einfluss auf diesen Effekt haben (für einen Überblick vgl. Metaanalyse von Baas et al. 2008). So fördern positive Gefühlszustände, welche mit erhöhter physiologischer Aktivierung einhergehen (z. B. Freude), die Kreativität; positive, mit niedriger physiologischer Aktivierung verbundene Gefühlszustände (z. B. Entspannung), hingegen nicht.

  5. 5.

    Dieser Einfluss der Stimmung auf die Erinnerung wird in der Literatur meist vereinfacht als „zustandsabhängige Erinnerung“ geführt. Da zustandsabhängige Erinnerung aber beispielsweise auch in Abhängigkeit vom Erregungslevel gezeigt werden konnte, verwenden wir hier eine differenziertere Bezeichnung.

  6. 6.

    Zwei weitere Gruppen sahen keinen Film, d. h., ihre Stimmung wurde nicht manipuliert. Allerdings erhielt eine dieser beiden Gruppen eine Hilfestellung, indem die Reißnägel zu Beginn nicht in, sondern neben der Schachtel lagen und die Schachtel somit leichter unabhängig von ihrer Funktion als Behältnis gesehen werden konnte. Mit dieser Hilfestellung löste ein ähnlich hoher Prozentsatz (83 %) wie bei den Teilnehmern in guter Stimmung (Gruppe B) die Aufgabe; ohne Hilfestellung war der Prozentsatz ähnlich niedrig (13 %) wie in Gruppe A.

  7. 7.

    Auch die Replikationsstudien von Yap et al. (2017, S. 942) hatten nicht das Ziel, den Mood-as-Information-Ansatz zu untersuchen: „[…] our goal is not to test the mood-as-information-theory.“

  8. 8.

    Der Begriff „emotionale Ansteckung“ ist insofern irreführend, als über emotionale Ansteckung nur Stimmungen und keine Emotionen ausgelöst werden können. Emotionen benötigen per definitionem ein Objekt, auf das sie gerichtet sind, und dieses kann über emotionale Ansteckung nicht mit vermittelt werden.

  9. 9.

    Wir sprechen hier von Affekt, da dieser Prozess der Differenzierung neben Emotionen auch auf Stimmungen zutrifft.

Literatur

  • Adelmann, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B. (1989). Facial efference and the experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 249–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adiga, A., Birger, J., Carter, A., & Feldman, A. (2001). Sudden impact. Money, 30, 257–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alfano, M. S., Joiner, T. E., & Perry, M. (1994). Attributional style: A mediator of the shyness-depression relationship? Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 287–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A. (1999). Attributional style, depression, and loneliness: A cross-cultural comparison of American and Chinese students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 482–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality development. New York: Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 104–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, L. M., Best, M. R., & Domjan, M. (1977). Learning mechanisms in food selection. Waco: Baylor University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batsell, W. R., Jr., & Brown, A. S. (1998). Human flavor-aversion learning: A comparison of traditional aversions and cognitive aversions. Learning and Motivation, 29, 383–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1995). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. In J. Mehler & S. Franck (Hrsg.), Cognition on cognition (S. 3–11). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J., Dalgleish, T., & Yule, W. (2004). Memory for emotional stories in high and low depressed children. Memory, 12, 214–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H. (2001). Mood and the use of general knowledge structures. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Hrsg.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (S. 9–26). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (1995). Affective states and the influence of activated general knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 766–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (S. 65–84). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Igou, E. R. (2005). Mood and the use of general knowledge structures in judgment and decision making. In T. Betsch & S. Haberstroh (Hrsg.), The routines of decision making (S. 193–210). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, C., & Wölk, M. (1996a). Mood and the use of scripts: Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Schwarz, N., & Wieland, R. (1996b). Mood and the impact of category membership and individuating information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 935–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumberg, S. H., & Izard, C. E. (1985). Affective and cognitive characteristics of depression in 10- and 11-year-old children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 194–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 621–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bone, P. F., & Ellen, P. S. (1999). Scents in the marketplace: Explaining a fraction of olfaction. Journal of Retailing, 75, 243–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In S. A. Christianson (Hrsg.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (S. 3–31). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2001). Mood and social memory. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (S. 95–120). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Mayer, J. D. (1985). Failure to replicate mood-dependent retrieval. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 39–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Mayer, J. D. (1989). In search of mooddependent retrieval. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 121–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., Monteiro, K. P., & Gilligan, S. G. (1978). Emotional mood as a context for learning and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 573–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., Gilligan, S. G., & Monteiro, K. P. (1981). Selectivity of learning caused by affective states. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 451–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. D., & Taylor, S. E. (1986). Affect and the processing of personal information: Evidence for moodactivated self-schemata. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 436–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1995). A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 4, 410–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., van Baaren, R. B., & Bargh, J. A. (2006). Linking automatic evaluation to mood and information processing style: Consequences for experienced affect, impression formation, and stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 70–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, M. S., Milberg, S., & Ross, J. (1983). Arousal cues arousal-related material in memory: Implications for understanding effects of mood on memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 633–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Parrott, W. G. (1991). Moods and their vicissitudes: Thoughts and feelings as information. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Emotion and social judgments. International series in experimental social psychology (S. 107–123). Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Hrsg.), Handbook of social cognition (S. 324–417). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cote, S. (2005). Reconciling the feelings-as-information and hedonic contingency models of how mood influences systematic information processing. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1656–1679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: The preferential detection of negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14, 14–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dill, J. C., & Anderson, C. A. (1999). Loneliness, shyness, and depression: The etiology and interrelationships of everyday problems in living. In T. Joiner & J. C. Coyne (Hrsg.), The interactional nature of depression: Advances in interpersonal approaches. Washington, DC: APA Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11, 86–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Grunedal, S. (2002). Facial reactions to emotional stimuli: Automatically controlled emotional responses. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 449–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domachowska, I., Heitmann, C., Deutsch, R., Goschke, T., Scherbaum, S., & Bolte, A. (2016). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention: Registered replication report of Gable and Harmon-Jones (2008). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 50–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58, 113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, K., & Bryan, T. S. (1997). Judgmental biases produced by instructions to disregard: The (paradoxical) case of emotional information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 849–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlichman, H., & Halpern, J. N. (1988). Affect and memory: Effects of pleasant and unpleasant odors on retrieval of happy and unhappy memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 769–779.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E. (1995). Mood as a mediator of place dependent memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 293–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E., & Macaulay, D. (2000a). Are real moods required to reveal mood-congruent and mood-dependent memory? Psychological Science, 11, 244–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E., & Macaulay, D. (2000b). Studies in emotion and social interaction, second series. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (S. 109–130). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E., Macaulay, D., & Ryan, L. (1994). Mood dependent memory for events of the personal past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 201–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erber, R., & Erber, M. W. (1994). Beyond mood and social judgment: Mood incongruent recall and mood regulation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 79–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). Positive affect improves creative problem solving and influences reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 285–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feddersen, J., Metcalfe, R., & Wooden, M. (2012). Subjective well-being: Weather matters; climate doesn’t (Melbourne Small Telescopes 569 Institute Working Paper Series, 25/2012). Melbourne, Victoria, Australia: University of Melbourne. Abgerufen von http://web.archive.org/web/20150107020727/http://melbourneinstitute.com/downloads/working_paper_series/wp2012n25.pdf.

  • Fedorikhin, A., & Cole, C. A. (2004). Mood effects on attitudes, perceived risk and choice: Moderators and mediators. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 2–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (1990). Mood-dependent selectivity in social cognition. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Hrsg.), European review of social psychology (Bd. 1, S. 1–32). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (1991). On the task, the measures and the mood in research on affect and social cognition. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Emotion and social judgments (S. 83–104). Elmsford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (2001a). Affective influences on social information processing. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (S. 163–185). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (2001b). Affective states trigger processes of assimilation and accommodation. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Hrsg.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (S. 85–98). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K., & Beier, S. (2014). Affect and cognitive processes in educational contexts. In P. A. Alexander, R. Pekrun, & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Hrsg.), International handbook of emotions in education (S. 36–55). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K., Asbeck, J., & Nickel, S. (1991). Mood and constructive memory effects on social judgement. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 363–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K., Nickel, S., Muehlfriedel, T., & Unkelbach, C. (2001). Is mood congruency an effect of genuine memory or response bias? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 201–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K., Nickel, S., Asbeck, J., & Pagel, U. (2003). Mood and the generation effect. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 585–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. P. (2000). I like it, but I’m not sure why: Can evaluative conditioning occur without conscious awareness? Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 9, 13–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2017). Social cognition: From brains to culture (3. Aufl.). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1993). On making sense of odd couples: Mood effects on the perception of mismatched relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 59–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1994). The role of emotion in social judgments: An introductory review and an affect infusion model (AIM). European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995a). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995b). Strange couples: Mood effects on judgments and memory about prototypical and atypical relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 747–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1998). On being happy and mistaken: Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 318–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (Hrsg.). (2001). Handbook of affect and Social cognition. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 53–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1988). Affect in social judgments. Australian Journal of Psychology, 40, 125–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & Eich, E. (2012). Affective influences on cognition: Mood congruence, mood dependence, and mood effects on processing styles. In I. B. Weiner, A. F. Healy, & R. W. Proctor (Hrsg.), Handbook of psychology, volume 4, experimental psychology (2 Aufl., S. 61–82). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & Moylan, S. (1987). After the movies: Transient mood and social judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 467–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., & Dannenberg, L. (2010). GLOMOsys: A systems account of global versus local processing. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 175–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franck, D. (1997). Verhaltensbiologie. Stuttgart: Thieme.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedlander, S., Traylor, J. A., & Weiss, D. S. (1986). Depressive symptoms and attributional style in children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 442–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R., & Förster, J. (2002). The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on creative cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R., & Förster, J. (2010). Implicit affective cues and attentional tuning: An integrativereview. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 875–893.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotions episodes and emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Hrsg.), Handbook of emotions (S. 381–403). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K. (2004). Permission to seek freely? The effect of happy and sad moods on generating old and new ideas. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 215–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gay, V. (2001). When bad gets worse. Brandweek, 42, SR3–SR4.

    Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M. (1996). Trait and state affect. In K. R. Murphy (Hrsg.), Individual differences and behavior in organizations (S. 145). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grawitch, M. J., Munz, D. C., & Kramer, T. J. (2003). Effects of member mood states on creative performance in temporary workgroups. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7, 41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greifeneder, R., Bless, H., & Pham, M. T. (2011). When do people rely on affective and cognitive feelings in judgment? A review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 107–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T. & Rapson, R. L. (1992). Primitive emotional contagion. In M. S. Clark (Hrsg.), Emotion and social behavior. Review of personality and social psychology (Bd. 14, S. 151–177).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 96–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heimpel, S. A., Wood, J. V., Marshall, M. A., & Brown, J. D. (2002). Do people with low self-esteem really want to feel better? Self-esteem differences in motivation to repair negative moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 128–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, G., & Fiedler, K. (1994). Affective and cognitive influences in a social dilemma game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 131–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1998). The aboutness principle: A pervasive influence on human inference. Social Cognition, 16, 173–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, E., Carlson, J. G., & Chemtob, C. (1990). The effect of power on susceptibility to emotional contagion. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, F., Beckmann, S. C., & Herrmann, A. (2004). Means-end analysis: Does the affective state influence information processing style? Psychology and Marketing, 21, 715–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huntsinger, J. R. (2013). Does emotion directly tune the scope of attention? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 265–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huntsinger, J. R., & Ray, C. (2016). A flexible influence of affective feelings on creative and analytic performance. Emotion, 16(6), 826–837. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000188

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huntsinger, J. R., Isbell, L., & Clore, G. L. (2014). The affective control of thought: Malleable, not fixed. Psychological Review, 141, 600–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. M. (2004). Not all happy people are lazy or stupid: Evidence of systematic processing in happy moods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 341–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. M., Burns, K. C., & Haar, T. (2005). The role of affect on the search for global and specific target information. Social Cognition, 23, 529–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Hrsg.), Handbook of social cognition (Bd. 20, S. 179–236). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206–1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Niedenthal, P. M., & Cantor, N. (1992). An influence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 65–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., & Magaro, P. A. (1987). Effects of mood and severity on memory processes in depression and mania. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 28–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 437–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapfhammer, H.-P., Huppert, D., Grill, E., Fitz, W., & Brandt, T. (2015). Visual height intolerance and acrophobia: Clinical characteristics and comorbidity patterns. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265(5), 375–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kavanagh, D. J., & Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and sadness on perceived capabilities. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 507–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, W. L., & Creeley, C. E. (2003). State-dependent memory effects using caffeine and placebo do not extend to metamemory. Journal of General Psychology, 130, 70–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimchi, R., & Palmer, S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 521–535.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2000). Affect and stereotypic thinking: A test of the mood-and-general-knowledge-model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1587–1597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 475–486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laird, J. D., Cuniff, M., Sheehan, K., Shulman, D., & Strum, G. (1989). Emotion specific effects of facial expressions on memory for life events. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 87–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laird, J. D., Alibozak, T., Davainis, D., Deignan, K., Fontanella, K., Hong, J., Levy, B., & Pacheco, C. (1994). Individual differences in the effects of spontaneous mimicry on emotional contagion. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 231–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, A. J., Craske, M. G., Brown, M., & Ghaneian, A. (2001). Fear-related state dependent memory. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 695–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation: Conceptual and empirical relations. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 16, 175–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J., & Gonzalez, R. M. (2005). Forecasting one’s future based on fleeting subjective experiences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 454–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., & Rosenbaum, M. (1987). Recall of parental behavior by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and nondepressives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 611–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, G. G., & Lishman, W. A. (1975). Effect of depression on the speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychological Medicine, 5, 173–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., & Hsee, C. K. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lous, A. M., de Wit, C. A. M., De Bruyn, E. E. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2002). Depression markers in young children’s play: A comparison between depressed and nondepressed 3-to 6-year-olds in various play situations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 1029–1038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Lawless, N. M. (2013). Does life seem better on a sunny day? Examining the association between daily weather conditions and life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 872–884.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, F., McConkey, K. M., Bryant, R. A., & Barnier, A. J. (2000). Specific autobiographical memory following hypnotically induced mood state. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 48, 361–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., Ward, D. W., Achee, J. W., & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implications of their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 317–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers, B. A., & McGraw, A. P. (2001). Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 210–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, C., & Hardman, E. (1998). State-dependent memory produced by aerobic exercise. Ergonomics, 41, 20–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 355–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, R., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2005). Mood congruence in childhood and recent autobiographical memory. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 981–998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, R. (2000). The causal influences of attributions on emotions: A procedural priming approach. Psychological Science, 11, 179–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). „Mood contagion“: The automatic transfer of mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 211–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newton, D. P. (2013). Moods, emotions and creative thinking: A framework for teaching. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 34–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., & Halberstadt, J. H. (2000). Grounding categories in emotional response. In J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in Social cognition (S. 357–386). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Setterlund, M. B. (1997). Being happy and seeing „happy“: Emotional state mediates visual word recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 403–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, S. L., & Sarason, I. G. (1981). Emotion, personality, and selective attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 945–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 405–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1998). The effect of music on atmosphere and purchase intentions in a cafeteria. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 2254–2273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). „Unconscious anxiety“: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Omdahl, B. L. (1995). Cognitive appraisal, emotion, and empathy. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Mood and heuristics: The influence of happy and sad states on sensitivity and bias in stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1005–1023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, W. G., & Sabini, J. (1990). Mood and memory under natural conditions: Evidence for mood incongruent recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 321–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Gärling, T., & Slovic, P. (2006). Affect and decision making: A “hot” topic. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 79–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2004). Gut reactions: A perceptual theory of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley, A. M., & Clifford, B. R. (2004). The effects of anxious mood induction on suggestibility to misleading post-event information. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 233–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roschk, H., Loureiro, S. M. C., & Breitsohl, J. (2017). Calibrating 30 years of experimental research: A meta-analysis of the atmospheric effects of music, scent, and color. Journal of Retailing, 93, 228–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinations of emotion: Constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 241–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of sympathic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 703–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz-Caballero, J. A., & Gonzalez, P. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory bias in depressed and nondepressed subjects. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 555–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz-Caballero, J. A., & Gonzalez, P. (1997). Effects of level of processing on implicit and explicit memory in depressed mood. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rusting, C. L., & DeHart, T. (2000). Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: Consequences for mood-congruent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 737–752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P., & Birnbaum, D. (1989). Influence of mood on health-relevant cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 539–551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1988). Cognitive antecedents of emotion. In V. L. Hamilton, G. H. Bower, & N. H. Frijda (Hrsg.), Cognitive perspectives on emotion and motivation (S. 89–126). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., & Laird, J. D. (2003). Keep smiling: Enduring effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience and memory. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 787–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Information and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. Sorrentino (Hrsg.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Bd. 2, S. 527–561). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (1996). Feelings and their motivational implications: Moods and the action sequence. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Hrsg.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (S. 119–145). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? Informative functions of affective states. In K. Fiedler & J. P. Forgas (Hrsg.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (S. 44–62). Toronto: Hogrefe International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Hrsg.), Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles (S. 433–465). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In A.Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Hrsg.), Social psychology. Handbook of basic principles (2. Aufl., S. 385–407). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2016). Evaluating psychological research requires more than attention to the N: A comment on Simonsohn’s (2015) „Small telescopes”. Psychological Science, 27(10), 1407–1409. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616653102

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Strack, F., Kommer, D., & Wagner, D. (1987). Soccer, rooms, and the quality of your life: Mood effects on judgments of satisfaction with life in general and with specific domains. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 69–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E., Abramson, L. Y., Semmel, A., & von Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). Investment behavior and the negative side of emotion. Psychological Science, 16, 435–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 29–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simonsohn, U. (2015). Rain & happiness: Why didn’t Schwarz & Clore (1983) ‚replicate‘? [Web log post]. Abgerufen von http://datacolada.org/43.

  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Hrsg.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (S. 397–420). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). Patterns of appraisal and emotion related to taking an exam. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 475–488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1990). Emotion and adaptation. In L. A. Pervin (Hrsg.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (S. 609–637). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1092–1107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). With Sadness conies accuracy; with happiness, false memory: mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science, 16, 785–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F. (2017). From Data to Truth in Psychological Science. A Personal Perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 702. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00702

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460–1469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A non-obtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroebe, W., & Strack, F. (2014). The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 59–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 248–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones & L. Feldman Barrett (Hrsg.), Handbook of emotions (3. Aufl., S. 114–137). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turley, L. W., & Milliman, R. E. (2000). Atmospheric effects on shopping behavior: A review of the experimental evidence. Journal of Business Research, 49, 193–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (1987). Conditions leading to value-expressive effects in judgments of procedural justice: A test of four models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 333–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K. (2003). On the subjective quality of social justice: The role of affect as information in the psychology of justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 482–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E., Jr. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verduyn, P., Delaveau, P., Rotgé, J. Y., Fossati, P., & Van Mechelen, I. (2015). Determinants of emotion duration and underlying psychological and neural mechanisms. Emotion Review, 7, 330–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walther, E. (2002). Guilty by mere association: Evaluative conditioning and the spreading attitude effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 919–934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Gold, D. B. (1995). Emotional and cognitive effects of suppressing thoughts of a past relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 782–792.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., & Handel, S. J. (1985). A cognition-emotion-action sequence: Anticipated emotional consequences of causal attributions and reported communication strategy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 102–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Graham, S., & Chandler, C. (1982a). Pity, anger, and guilt: An attributional analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 226–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Graham, S., Stern, P., & Lawson, M. E. (1982b). Using affective cues to infer causal thoughts. Developmental Psychology, 18, 278–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes, and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Hrsg.), Research in organizational behavior: An annual series of analytical essays and critical reviews (Bd. 18, S. 1–74). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J., & Mischel, W. (1982). Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 901–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yap, S. C. Y., Wortman, J., Anusic, I., Baker, S. G., Scherer, L. D., Donnellan, M. B., & Lucas, R. E. (2017). The effect of mood on judgments of subjective well-being: Nine tests of the judgment model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 939–961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zenasni, F., & Lubart, T. (2002). Effects of mood states on creativity. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain and Cognition, 8, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lioba Werth .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer-Verlag GmbH Deutschland, ein Teil von Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Werth, L., Denzler, M., Mayer, J. (2020). Denken und Fühlen. In: Sozialpsychologie – Das Individuum im sozialen Kontext. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-53897-5_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-53897-5_4

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-53896-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-53897-5

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics