Skip to main content

Theorien der kognitiven Entwicklung

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Zusammenfassung

Ein sieben Monate alter Junge sitzt auf dem Schoß seines Vaters und ist von dessen Brille fasziniert, greift nach einem der Bügel und zieht daran. Der Vater sagt „Au!“, und der Junge lässt los, fasst dann aber erneut hin und zieht an der Brille. Das bringt den Vater dazu, sich zu fragen, wie er die Brillengläser in Sicherheit bringen kann, ohne dass das Kind zu weinen anfängt. Glücklicherweise kommt der Vater, ein Entwicklungspsychologe, schnell auf die Idee, dass Jean Piagets Theorie der kognitiven Entwicklung eine einfache Lösung vorschlägt: Verbirg die Brille hinter dem Rücken! Nach Piagets Theorie sollte das Entfernen der Brille aus dem Sichtfeld ein Baby diesen Alters dazu bringen, sich so zu verhalten, als ob die Brille nicht mehr existierte. Die Strategie funktioniert genau wie geplant; nachdem der Vater die Brille aus dem Sichtfeld entfernt hat, zeigt der Junge kein weiteres Interesse daran und richtet seine Aufmerksamkeit woanders hin. Der Vater dankt Piaget im Stillen.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Hier und im weiteren Verlauf dieses Abschnitts verwenden wir den Ausdruck „Informationsverarbeitungstheorien“ im Plural und nicht im Singular, weil diese Theorien eine Vielzahl verwandter Ansätze umfassen und nicht die einheitlichen Vorstellungen eines einzelnen Theoretikers wie Piaget zum Ausdruck bringen. Aus demselben Grund werden wir in den nachfolgenden Abschnitten von „soziokulturellen Theorien“ sprechen.

Literatur

  • Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., & Deckner, D. F. (2004). The development of symbol-infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75, 1171–1187. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00732.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62(3, Serial No. 251), 1–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., & Berger, S. E. (2011). Physical and motor development. In M. H. Bornstein, & M. E. Lamb (Hrsg.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (6. Aufl. S. 314–302). New York, NY: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 59–65. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00044.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, E., Blaney, N., Stephin, C., Sikes, J., & Snapp, M. (1978). The jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. F. (1987). Functional and cognitive memory theory: An overview of some key issues. In D. S. Gorfein, & R. R. Hoffman (Hrsg.), Memory and learning: The Ebbinghaus Centennial Conference (S. 387–395). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1987). Young infants’ reasoning about the physical and spatial properties of a hidden object. Cognitive Development, 2, 179–200. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(87)90043-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1993). The object concept revisited: New directions in the investigation of infants’ physical knowledge. In C. E. Granrud (Hrsg.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (Bd. 23, S. 265–315). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, S. T., Friedman, O., & Leslie, A. M. (2010). The opposites task: Using general rules to test cognitive flexibility in preschoolers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 240–254. doi:10.1080/15248371003699944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 874–890. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01577.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, C. A., Strough, J., Calderone, K., Meegan, S. P., & Sansone, C. (1997). Planning to prevent everyday problems from occurring. In S. L. Friedman, & E. K. Scholnick (Hrsg.), The developmental psychology of planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? (S. 209–236). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E. (1994). Why children talk to themselves. Scientific American, 271(5), 78–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blauw-Hospers, C. H., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2005). A systematic review of the effects of early intervention on motor development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 421–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A., & Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting children’s memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event unfolds. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 39–65. doi:10.1080/15248372.2003.9669682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 42, 189–201. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. Child Development, 79, 1016–1031. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01173.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35, 207–220. doi:10.1017/S030500090700829X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. (1997). Transforming schools into communities of thinking and learning about serious matters. American Psychologist, 52, 399–413. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, S. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). A brain-based account of the developmentof rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., Kermoian, R., & Zumbahlen, M. R. (1992). Socioemotional transformations in the family system following infant crawling onset. In N. Eisenberg, & R. A. Fabes (Hrsg.), Emotion and its regulation in early development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, (Bd. 55, S. 25–40). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1998). The development of conceptual structures. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Hrsg.), Cognition, perception, and language 5. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 2, S. 745–800). New York, NY: Wiley. W. Damon (Series Ed.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D., & Lawrence, J. A. (1993). Investigating the effects of planning aids on adults’ and adolescents’ organisation of a complex task. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 191–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z., & Siegler, R. (2000). Across the great divide: Bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers’ and older children’s thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65(2, Serial No. 261), 65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z., Mo, L., & Honomichl, R. (2004). Having the memory of an elephant: Long-term retrieval and the use of analogues in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 415–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., & Ceci, S. J. (1987). Content knowledge: Its role, representation, and restructuring in memory development. In H. W. Reese (Hrsg.), Advances in child development and behavior (Bd. 20, S. 91–142). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clearfield, M. W., Dineva, E., Smith, L. B., Diedrich, F. J., & Thelen, E. (2009). Cue salience and infant perseverative reaching: Tests ofthe dynamic field theory. Developmental Science, 12, 26–40. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00769.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Collie, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Deferred imitation by 6- and 9-month old infants: More evidence for declarative memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 83–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, D. B., Knight, D. K., & Cross, D. R. (1997). Mothers’ and fathers’ scaffolding of their 2-year-olds during problem-solving and literacy interactions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 323–338. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1997.tb00524.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Nugent, L. D., Elliott, E. M., Ponomarev, I., & Saults, J. S. (1999). The role of attention in the development of short-term memory: Age differences in the verbal span of apprehension. Child Development, 70, 1082–1097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeMarie-Dreblow, D., & Miller, P. H. (1988). The development of children’s strategies for selective attention: Evidence for a transitional period. Child Development, 59, 1504–1513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N. (1995). Interference and inhibition in cognition: An historical perspective. In F. N. Dempster, & C. J. Brainerd (Hrsg.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (S. 3–16). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N., & Corkill, A. J. (1999). Interference and inhibition incognition and behavior: Unifying themes for educational psychology. EducationalPsychology Review, 11, 1–88. doi:10.1023/A:1021992632168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deák, G. O., Flom, R. A., & Pick, A. D. (2000). Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds’ joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology, 36, 511–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB¯. Child Development, 56, 868–883. doi:10.2307/1130099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Kirkham, N., & Amso, D. (2002). Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Developmental Psychology, 38, 352–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diedrich, F. J., Thelen, E., Smith, L. B., & Corbetta, D. (2000). Motor memory is a factor in infant perseverative errors. Developmental Science, 3, 479–494. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., & Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428–1446. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fallang, B., Saugstad, O. D., Grogaard, J., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2003). Kinematic quality of reaching movements in preterm infants. Pediatric Research, 53, 836–842. doi:10.1203/01.PDR.0000058925.94994.BC.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, D. (1987). A review of preschool conservation training: An analysis of analyses. Developmental Review, 7, 210–251. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(87)90013-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (2006). Dynamic development of action and thought. In R. M. Lerner (Hrsg.), Theoretical models of human development 6. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 1, S. 313–399). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1971). Stage-related properties of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 421–453. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(71)90025-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1982). On cognitive development. Child Development, 53, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M. (2001). The social context of cognitive development. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2006). Development of mathematical understanding. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Hrsg.), Cognition, perception, and language 6. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 2, S. 777–810). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1539–1552.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gershkoff-Stowe, L., Connell, B., & Smith, L. (2006). Priming over generalizations in two- and four-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 33, 461–486. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007562.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Göncü, A. (1993). Development of intersubjectivity in the dyadic play of preschoolers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 99–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 33, 295–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, K., & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1338–1348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heathcock, J. C., Lobo, M., & Galloway, J. C. (2008). Movement training advances the emergence of reaching in infants born at less than 33 weeks of gestational age: A randomized clinical trial. Physical Therapy, 88, 310–322. doi:10.2522/ptj.20070145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Hofsten, C. (2007). Action in development. Developmental Science, 10, 54–60. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00564.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Hofsten, C., Dahlström, E., & Fredriksson, Y. (2005). 12-monthold infants’ perception of attention direction in static video images. Infancy, 8, 217–231. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0803_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. A., Sosa, B., & Shapiro, L. R. (1997). Scripts and plans: The development of preschool children’s event knowledge and event planning. In S. L. Friedman, & E. K. Scholnick (Hrsg.), The developmental psychologyof planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? (S. 77–102). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2008). Conceptual change in naïve biology. In S. Vosniadou (Hrsg.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (S. 240–262). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence: An essay on the construction of formal operational structures. New York, NY: Basic Books. Deutsch 1980, Von der Logik des Kindes zur Logik des Heranwachsenden. Essay über die Ausformung der formalen operativen Strukturen. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. E., & Mervis, C. B. (1994). Microgenetic analysis of first steps in children’s acquisition of expertise on shorebirds. Developmental Psychology, 30, 418–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1984). The development of memory in children (2. Aufl.). NewYork, NY: Freeman. Deutsch 1992, Gedächtnisentwicklung bei Kindern. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1997). Processing time, imagery, and spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 67–78. doi:10.1006/jecp.1996.2337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D. (1978). Goal formation, planning, and learning by pre-school problem solvers or “My socks are in the dryer.” In R. Siegler (Hrsg.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (S. 181–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Franklin, S. (2006). The second decade: What develops (and how). In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Hrsg.), Cognition, perception, and language 6. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 2, S. 953–993). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Macro- and microdevelopmental research: Assumptions, research strategies, constraints, and utilities. In N. Granott, & J. Parziale (Hrsg.), Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning (S. 243–265). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Leichtman, M. D., Pillemer, D. B., Wang, Q., Koreishi, A., & Han, J. J. (2000). When Baby Maisy came to school: Mothers’ interview styles and preschoolers’ event memories. Cognitive Development, 15, 99–114. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00019-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Siegler, R. S., & Druyan, S. (1990). Misconceptions about motion: Development and training effects. Child Development, 61, 1544–1557. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02882.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Libertus, K., & Needham, A. (2010). Teach to reach: The effects of active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Research, 50, 2750–2757. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindberg, M. (1991). A taxonomy of suggestibility and eyewitness memory: Age, memory process, and focus of analysis. In J. Doris (Hrsg.), The suggestibility of children’s recollection: Implications for eyewitness memory (S. 47–55). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lindberg, M. A. (1980). Is knowledge base development a necessary and sufficient condition for memory development? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 401–410. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(80)90046-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linkletter, A. (1957). Kids say the darndest things! Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobo, M. A., Galloway, J. C., & Savelsbergh, G. J. (2004). General and task-related experiences affect early object interaction. Child Development, 75, 1268–1281. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00738.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Garver, K. E., Urban, T. A., Lazar, N. A., & Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development, 75, 1357–1372. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. H., Choy, M., Pullman, S., & Meng, Z. (2004). Corticospinal system development depends on motor experience. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2122–2132. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4616-03.2004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2012). Self-regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 136–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, F., & Salmon, K. (2004). The time to talk: The influence of the timing of adult-child talk on children’s event memory. Child Development, 75, 669–686. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00700.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. (1984). Child as the measurer of all things: Measurement of procedures and the development of quantitative concepts. In C. Sophian (Hrsg.), Origins of cognitive skills: The Eighteenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (S. 193–228). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. H. (2011). Theories of developmental psychology (5. Aufl.). NewYork, NY: Worth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. H., & Coyle, T. R. (1999). Developmental change: Lessons from microgenesis. In E. K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S. A. Gelman, & P. H. Miller (Hrsg.), Conceptual development: Piaget’s legacy (S. 209–239). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. H., & Seier, W. L. (1994). Strategy utilization deficiencies in children: When, where, and why. In H. W. Reese (Hrsg.), Advances in child development and behavior (Bd. 25, S. 108–156). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Willpower in a cognitive affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In K. D. Vohs, & R. F. Baumeister (Hrsg.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2. Aufl. S. 99–129). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-controlpredicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 2693–2698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (2008). The development of gaze following. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 66–70. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00052.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munakata, Y., Lerd, S.A., Chatham, C.H., Depue, B.E., Banich, M.T. & O’Reilly, R.C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Coginitive Neuroscience, 15, 453–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 71–77. doi:10.1177/0963721412436807.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regu­lation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field, & N. A. Fox (Hrsg.), Social Perception in Infants (S. 177–197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Association for the Education of Young Children (2011). 2010 NAEYC Standards for Initial & Advanced Early Childhood Professional Preparation Programs. http://www.naeyc.org/ncate/files/ncate/file/faculty/Standards/NAEYC%20Initial%20and%20Advanced%20Standards%203_2012.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  • Needham, A., Barrett, T., & Peterman, K. (2002). A pick-me-up for infants’ exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using “sticky mittens” enhances young infants’ object exploration skills. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 279–295. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00097-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, C. A., Thomas, K. M., & de Haan, M. (2013). Neural bases of cognitive development. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Hrsg.), Cognition, perception, and language 6. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 2, S. 3–57). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486–511. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Opfer, J. E., & Gelman, S. A. (2001). Children’s and adults’ models for predicting teleological action: The development of a biology-based model. Child Development, 72, 1367–1381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2004). Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 301–332. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.01.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T. (2010). Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: Myelinor axon? Brain and Cognition, 72, 26–35. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., Chudowsky, N., & Glaser, R. (Hrsg.). (2001). Knowing what students know: The science and design of educational assessment. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1969). Nachahmung, Spiel und Traum. Stuttgart: Klett. Original 1945, La formation du symbole chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1972). Psychology and epistemology: Towards a theory of knowledge, Hamondsworth, England: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1973a). Nachahmung, Spiel und Traum. Stuttgart: Klett. Original 1945, La formation du symbole chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1973b). Psychology and epistemology. New York: W. W. Norton. Original 1970, Psychologie et épistémologie. Paris: Gonthier Denoël

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1937/1998). Der Aufbau der Wirklichkeit beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. Original 1937, La construction du réel chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1981). Urteil und Denkprozeß beim Kind. Frankfurt/M.: Ullstein. Original 1928, Le juge­ment et le raisonnement chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1994). Die Entwicklung des Zahlbegriffs beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. Original 1941, La genese du nombre chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1996). Das Erwachen der Intelligenz beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. Original 1948, La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1977). The child’s conception of space. In H. E. Gruber, & J. J. Vonèche (Hrsg.), The essential Piaget (S. 576–642). New York: Basic Books. Nachdruck von The child’s conception of space von Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.), 1956, London, England: Routledge & K. Paul

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M. (1995). Relations between children’s overestimation of their physical abilities and accident proneness. Developmental Psychology, 31, 866–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., Kearney, J. K., & Cremer, J. F. (2004). Children’s perception of gap affordances: Bicycling across traffic-filled intersections in an immersive virtual environment. Child Development, 75, 1243–1253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, M. W., Kerig, P., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (1988). Mothers and fathers teaching 3-year-olds: Authoritative parenting and adult scaffolding of young children’s learning. Developmental Psychology, 24, 832–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., & Hilden, K. (2006). Cognitive strategies: Production deficiencies and successful strategy instruction everywhere. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Hrsg.), Cognition, perception, and language 6. Aufl. Handbookof child psychology, (Bd. 2, S. 511–556). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Development, 79, 375–394. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01131.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li-Grining, C., Zhai, F., Metzger, M. W., & Solomon, B. (2009). Targeting children’s behavior problems in preschool classrooms: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 302–316. doi:10.1037/a0015302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li-Grining, C., Zhai, F., Bub, K., & Pressler, E. (2011). CSRP’s Impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development, 82, 362–378. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, J. M., & Squire, L. R. (1998). Retrograde amnesia for facts and events: Findings from four new cases. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 3943–3954.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1985). Language acquisition as increasing linguistic structuring of experience and symbolic behavior control. In J. V. Wertsch (Hrsg.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (S. 183–204). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2011). Modeling a cascade of effects: The role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic achievement. Developmental Science, 14, 1161–1175. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01068.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 74–81. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, L. K., & Horst, J. S. (2008). Confronting complexity: Insights from the details of behavior over multiple timescales. Developmental Science, 11, 209–215. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00667.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B., Guberman, S. R., & Gearhart, M. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52(2, Serial No. 216), 52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1998). Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Developmental Science, 1, 291–297. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (2011). Memory development in childhood. In U. Goswami (Hrsg.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2. Aufl. S. 347–376). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Körkel, J., & Weinert, F. E. (1989). Domain-specific knowledge and memory performance: A comparison of high- and low aptitude children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 306–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutte, A. R., Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003). Testing the dynamic field theory: Working memory for locations becomes more spatially precise over development. Child Development, 74, 1393–1417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1976). The effects of simple necessity and sufficiency relationships on children’s causal inferences. Child Development, 47, 1058–1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children’s addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 250–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of number conservation. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 225–273. doi:10.1006/cogp.1995.1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learning. In D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (Hrsg.), Cognition, perception, and language 6. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 2, S. 464–510). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (1998). Developmental differences in rule learning: A microgenetic analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 273–310. doi:10.1006/cogp.1998.0686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games – but not circular ones – improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 545–560. doi:10.1037/a0014239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, T. J., & Klahr, D. (1995). A computational theory of children’s learning about number conservation. In T. J. Simon, & G. S. Halford (Hrsg.), Developing cognitive competence: New approaches to process modeling (S. 315–353). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B. (2005). Action alters shape categories. Cognitive Science, 29, 665–679. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235–260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J. P., Clearfield, M., Corbetta, D., Ulrich, B., Buchanan, P., & Schöner, G. (2006). Moving toward a grand theory of development: In memory of Esther Thelen. Child Development, 77, 1521–1538. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00955.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spilich, G. J., Vesonder, G. T., Chiesi, H. L., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 275–290. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90155-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenbeek, H., & van Geert, P. (2008). An empirical validation of a dynamic systems model of interaction: Do children of different sociometric statuses differ in their dyadic play? Developmental Science, 11, 253–281. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00655.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Development, 57, 646–659.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thatcher, R. W. (1992). Cyclic cortical reorganization during early childhood. Brain and Cognition, 20, 24–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (2001). Dynamic mechanisms of change in early perceptual motor development. In J. L. McClelland, & R. Siegler (Hrsg.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives (S. 161–184). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Corbetta, D. (1994). Exploration and selection in the early acquisition of skill. International Review of Neurobiology, 37, 75–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In R. M. Lerner (Hrsg.), Theoretical models of human development 5. Aufl. Handbook of child psychology, (Bd. 1, S. 563–634). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. W. Damon (Series Ed.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., Corbetta, D., Kamm, K., Spencer, J. P., Schneider, K., & Zernicke, R. F. (1993). The transition to reaching: Mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. Child Development, 64, 1058–1098.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2001). Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life. In M. Bowerman, & S. C. Levinson (Hrsg.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (S. 132–158). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Q. (2007). “Remember when you got the big, big bulldozer?” Mother-child reminiscing over time and across cultures. Social Cognition, 25, 455–471. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.4.455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, H. S. (1989). Problem-solving at two: A year-long naturalistic study of two children Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, MO, April. Kansas City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. P., & Kipp, K. (1998). The development of efficient inhibition: Evidence from directed-forgetting tasks. Developmental Review, 18, 86–123. doi:10.1006/drev.1997.0445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winsler, A., De Leon, J. R., Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behaviour. Journal of Child Language, 30, 583–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, B. C., Daw, N. D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Striatal activity underlies novelty-based choice in humans. Neuron, 58, 967–973. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.027.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. (1986). Aspects of teaching and learning. In M. Richards, & P. Light (Hrsg.), Children of social worlds: Development in a social context (S. 191–212). Cambridge, England: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 17, 89–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wygotski, L. S. (1934/2002). Denken und Sprechen. Weinheim und Basel: Beltz. Original Thought and language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3, Serial No. 274), 11–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Spinazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Developmental Psychology, 34, 203–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Siegler .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Siegler, R., Eisenberg, N., DeLoache, J., Saffran, J. (2016). Theorien der kognitiven Entwicklung. In: Pauen, S. (eds) Entwicklungspsychologie im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-47028-2_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-47028-2_4

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-47027-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-47028-2

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics