Skip to main content

Uniqueness and Possession: Typological Evidence for Type Shifts in Nominal Determination

  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
  • 735 Accesses

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Computer Science ((LNTCS,volume 8984))

Abstract

This paper highlights the analogy of definiteness and possession by utilising the distinction between semantic and pragmatic uniqueness as outlined in Löbner’s (2011) Concept Type and Determination approach. Assuming, on the basis of the features [\(\pm \) unique] and [\(\pm \) relational], a classification into the four logical types sortal, relational, individual, and functional concept, nouns will be used either in congruence with or deviating from their underlying type. I present evidence from Germanic and Mayan languages for the following claims: (1) noun uses that deviate from the underlying type tend to be reflected by overt morphology; (2) in article split languages, phonologically ‘strong’ forms indicate pragmatic uniqueness, thus, denote a function from [\(-\) unique] to [+ unique], whereas ‘weak’ forms tend to be semantically redundant. Regarding possession, ‘alienable’ morphology denotes a function from non-relational to relational (pragmatic possession), whereas ‘inalienable’ morphology is restricted to semantic possession. Overall, split systems imply a strong correlation between conceptual markedness and morphosyntactic markedness.

The work reported here was carried out in the research Unit FOR 600 Functional concepts and frames, and subsequently in the Collaborative Research Centre SFB 991 The Structure of Representation in Language, Cognition and Science, both sponsored by the German Research Foundation (DFG). For comments and discussion I would like to thank Adrian Czardybon, Thomas Gamerschlag, Corinna Handschuh, Lisa Hofmann, Sebastian Löbner, and Chris Lucas. Special thanks go to Doris Gerland and Jenny Kohls for their careful and critical reading of an earlier version, and to two anonymous referees for their extremely valuable and detailed comments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For controversial cases such as ‘configurational uses’ see Löbner (2011: 298) and references there. See also Carlson & Sussman (2005) on ‘weak definites’ such as (go to) the store, as well as Coppock & Beaver (2012) on anti-uniqueness effects of predicative definites and also of argumental definites under negation.

  2. 2.

    In the glosses, I use the following abbreviations of grammatical categories: acc ‘accusative’, aux ‘auxiliary’, comp ‘complementiser’, cop ‘copula’, dat ‘dative’, def ‘definite article’, dem ‘demonstrative pronoun’, derel ‘de-relativisation’, di ‘distal determination’, e ‘ergative’, ep ‘epenthetic consonant’, f ‘feminine’, gen ‘genitive’, imp ‘imperative’, inf ‘infinitive’, loc ‘locative’, m ‘masculine’, n ‘neuter’, neg ‘negation’, nom ‘nominative’, non3rd, ‘1st or 2nd person’, part ‘participle’, past ‘past tense’, pl ‘plural’, poss ‘relation of possession’, posscl ‘possessive classifier’, pres ‘present tense’, refl ‘reflexive pronoun’, rel ‘relative clause marker’, sg ‘singular’, str ‘strong article form’, superl ‘superlative’, wk ‘weak article form’ ; 1, 2 and 3 represent first, second and third person, respectively.

  3. 3.

    In Barker (2011: 1113) this distinction is labelled lexical vs. pragmatic interpretation; see also Vikner & Jensen (2002: 194–216) for a similar though not identical distinction.

  4. 4.

    See Bricker et al. & Po’ot (1998: 358f) for other suffixes with essentially the same function, as well as for further details concerning possession in Yucatec.

  5. 5.

    As in Yucatec, the possessor clitics belong to the ergative paradigm (‘set A’ in the Mayanist tradition).

  6. 6.

    In addition to vowel lengthening, some nouns undergo further regular vowel-related processes when they are possessed (cf. England 1983: 44). For example, the processes involved in n-paatzán=a ‘my sugarcane’ (with the unpossessed variant ptz’on) are stress assignment, prevocalic glottalisation, and reduction of unstressed vowels, hence the phonetic form

    figure h

    .

  7. 7.

    Another instance of de-relationisation comes from Teop (Western-Oceanic, Papua New Guinea). Ulrike Mosel (p.c.) informs me that the suffix -na serves the same function, as in sina-na mother-derel ‘(a) mother’. One more case in question is Cahuilla (Uto-Aztecan; Seiler 1983).

  8. 8.

    Strictly speaking, in the case of FNs (as opposed to RNs) it takes the combination of two shifts to arrive at an SC. One is de-relativisation as in (15), the other is [+ unique] \(\rightarrow \) [\(-\) unique] (‘de-functionalisation’, as it were) and will be briefly touched in 4.2. The effect of the two shifts is represented below in (18).

  9. 9.

    The notion ‘establishing relative clause’ goes back to Hawkins (1978, 138ff.) and will become relevant in 4.4.1. ‘DAA’ represents ‘definite associative anaphora’ (also known as ‘bridging’). Like other anaphora, DAAs are anchored by the referent of a previously mentioned NP; e.g\(\dots a\,house \dots the\,door\dots \). Non-lexical FCs come about by combining nouns (of any type) with ordinals or superlative adjectives and will be discussed in 4.3.1.

  10. 10.

    For these and other criteria see Himmelmann (1997).

  11. 11.

    Besides, there is a special article form that is found with proper names (like in Catalan), and moreover with pronouns. Note that Catalan and Maori are not unusual in featuring so-called preproprial articles. One source of the latter, e.g., in dialects of Norwegian, are third person forms of personal pronouns; cf. Matushansky (2008).

  12. 12.

    An anonymous reviewer (Reviewer 1) raises the question as to the exact nature of such a shift from \(e\) to \(\langle e,t\rangle \); specifically, if one should exclusively think of it as Partee’s (1986) IDENT, in which case there would be a problem with respect to a presupposition of existence (cf. Coppock & Beaver 2012: 533f). Essentially, we are dealing with a variety of shifts of which IDENT is only one. As a matter of fact, Partee (1986: 122) herself proposes another \(e\) to \(\langle e,t\rangle \) shift, labelled PRED, which returns properties from their entity-correlates. One other is, following Löbner (2011: 284f), a shift that is operative with proper names in predicative or indefinite use, as in He’s an Einstein. Yet another instance is a shift from an individual constant into a predicate, by way of making use of its descriptive contents, as it seems to be necessary, e.g., for a moon.

  13. 13.

    An anonymous reviewer (Reviewer 2) rightly points out that underlying SNs such as ‘umbrella’ and ‘dog ’ must be turned into an IC in order for the weak article to be available in cases such as (27). The reviewer asks why this shift is not associated with morphological marking. As a matter of fact, many, if not most, shifting operations are silent rather than overtly marked. Typically, they are lexically restricted and depend on world knowledge, as in the case of ‘weak definites’ ; other instances are IN \(\rightarrow \) SC shifts as discussed in note 13 and cases of polysemy. It appears that with regar to operations among nominal concept types, only the most productive ones tend to be overt. Note that these are mainly the ones that rely on contextual rather than lexical information: the appropriate context to identify the antecedent of an anaphoric NP (SC \(\rightarrow \) IC) or a non-lexical POSS relation (SN \(\rightarrow \) RC). Similarly, existential binding, which is involved in the operation [+relational] \(\rightarrow \) [-relational], does not require lexical information either. More discussion of the shifting operations in detail is found in Löbner (2011: 310–312).

  14. 14.

    Thanks to Adrian Czardybon for providing the glosses for the examples from Sorbian.

  15. 15.

    In contrast to Upper Silesian, in Upper Sorbian the article is also obligatory with non-lexical functional concepts (i.e., ICs and FCs that come about by ordinal numbers and superlatives, which comprises a function over the domain that is characterised by the noun predicate), provided the NP is the comment rather than the topic of the clause. Similarly, associative anaphora (DAAs) tend to be generally preceded by the article in Upper Sorbian (Breu 2004: 20, 41), whereas in Upper Silesian this tends to be restricted to part-whole DAAs Czardybon (2010: 30ff, 2014: 309ff). What this shows with respect to the language-specific cut-off points on the scale (19) is that for Upper Sorbian the obligatory use of articles is two steps further advanced than in Upper Silesian.

  16. 16.

    The example (31) is an excerpt from spontaneous conversation among two elderly dialect speakers, recorded and transcribed by Jennifer Kohls. I would like to thank her for permitting me to quote her data.

References

  • Barker, C.: Possessive Descriptions. CSLI Publications, Stanford (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, C.: Possessives and relational nouns. In: Maienborn, C., von Heusinger, K., Portner, P. (eds.) Semantics: An International Handbook Of Natural Language Meaning, pp. 1109–1130. De Gruyter, Berlin (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, W.: Maori. Routledge, London (1993)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boeder, W.: Klassizistische Sprachkompetenz: Der altgeorgische Artikel bei Sulchan-Saba Orbeliani. (Klasicisṭuri enobrivi kompeṭenturoba: Ʒveli Kartuli naçevari Sulxan-Saba Orbelianis “SibrƷne sicruisaši”). Enatmecnirebis sakitxebi / Linguistic Issues 2009(12), 140–163 (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  • Breu, W.: Der definite Artikel in der obersorbischen Umgangssprache. In: Krause, M., Sappok, C. (eds.) Slavistische Linguistik 2002. Referate des XXVIII. Konstanzer Slavistischen Arbeitstreffens. Sagner, München (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bricker, V.R., Po’ot Yah, E., Dzul Po’ot, O.: A Dictionary of the Maya Language as Spoken in Hocabá, Yucatán. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, G.N., Sussman, R.: Seemingly indefinite definites. In: Kepser, S., Reis, M. (eds.) Linguistic Evidence: Empirical, Theoretical and Computational Perspectives, pp. 71–86. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (2005)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chappell, H., McGregor, W.: The Grammar of Inalienability: A Typological Perspective on Body Part Terms and the Part Whole Relation. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (1996)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Coppock, E., David, B.: Weak Uniqueness: the only difference between definites and indefinites. In: Proceedings of SALT, vol. 22, pp. 527–544 (2012)

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, T.: Inalienable possession in paamese grammar. In: Chappell, H., McGregor, W. (eds.) The Grammar of Inalienability: A Typological Perspective on Body Part Terms and the Part Whole Relation, pp. 383–432. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Czardybon, A.: Die Verwendung des definiten Artikels im Oberschlesischen im Sprachvergleich. Master thesis, University of Düsseldorf (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, K.H.: Referenz, Sprechsituation und die bestimmten Artikel in einem nordfriesischen Dialekt (Fering). Nordfriisk Instituut, Bredstedt (1971)

    Google Scholar 

  • England, N.C.: A Grammar of Mam, a Mayan Language. University of Texas Press, Austin (1983)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J.A.: Definiteness and Indefiniteness. A Study In Reference And Grammaticality Prediction. Croom Helm, London (1978)

    Google Scholar 

  • Himmelmann, N.: Deiktikon, Artikel, Nominalphrase: Zur Emergenz syntaktischer Struktur. Niemeyer, Tübingen (1997)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hualde, J.: Ignacio: Catalan. Routledge, London (1992)

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E.L., Ebert, K.H.: A note on marking transparency and opacity. Linguist. Inquiry 4, 412–424 (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, C.: Possession in Yucatec Maya. Lincom Europa, Munich (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Löbner, S.: Concept types and determination. J. Semantics 4, 279–326 (1985)

    Google Scholar 

  • Löbner, S.: Concept types and determination. J. Seman. 28, 279–333 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, C.: Definiteness. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1999)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Matushansky, O.: On the linguistic complexity of proper names. Linguist. Philos. 31, 573–627 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J.: On alienable and inalienable possession. In: Shipley, W. (ed.) In Honor of Mary Haas. Haas Festival Conference on Native American Linguistics, pp. 557–609. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J.: Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1992)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A.: Definite Article Asymmetries and Concept Types: semantic and pragmatic uniqueness. In: Gamerschlag, T., Gerland, D., Osswald, R., Petersen, W. (eds.) Frames and Concept Types: Applications in Language and Philosophy, pp. 293–321. Springer, Dordrecht (2014)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, A., Corinna H.: Semantic Factors of Valence-Changing Processes with Nouns: Possession in the Mayan Languages. DGfS-Jahrestagung Mainz, (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  • Partee, B.H.: Noun phrase interpretation and type-shifting principles. In: Groenendijk, J., de Jongh, D., Stokhof, M. (eds.) Foundations of pragmatics and lexical semantics, pp. 115–143. Foris, Dordrecht (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  • Partee, B.H.: Borshev, Vladimir: Genitives, Relational Nouns, and Argument-Modifier Ambiguity. In: Lang, E., Maienborn, C., Fabricius-Hansen, C. (eds.) Modifying Adjuncts, pp. 67–112. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (2003)

    Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, H.: Possession as an Operational Dimension of Language. Narr, Tübingen (1983)

    Google Scholar 

  • Studler, R.: The morphology, syntax and semantics of definite determiners in Swiss German. In: Hofherr, P.C., Zribi-Hertz, A. (eds.) Crosslinguistic Studies on Noun Phrase Structure and Reference, pp. 143–171. Leiden, Brill (2014)

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, C.: On the grammar of body parts in koyukon athabaskan. In: Chappell, H., McGregor, W. (eds.) The Grammar of Inalienability: A Typological Perspective on Body Part Terms and the Part Whole Relation, 551–676. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiling-Herrwegen, A.: De kölsche Sproch. Kurzgrammatik Kölsch - Deutsch. Cologne, Bachem (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Tozzer, A. M.: A Maya Grammar. With Bibliography and Appraisement of the Works Noted. Cambridge, MA. Reprinted 1974 by Kraus Reprint. Millwood, N.Y. (1921)

    Google Scholar 

  • Vikner, C., Jensen, P.A.: A semantic analysis of the english genitive. interaction of lexical and formal semantics. Studia Linguistica 56, 191–226 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Albert Ortmann .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this paper

Cite this paper

Ortmann, A. (2015). Uniqueness and Possession: Typological Evidence for Type Shifts in Nominal Determination. In: Aher, M., Hole, D., Jeřábek, E., Kupke, C. (eds) Logic, Language, and Computation. TbiLLC 2013. Lecture Notes in Computer Science(), vol 8984. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-46906-4_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-46906-4_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-46905-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-46906-4

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics