Skip to main content

Human Problem Handling

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handling Societal Complexity

Abstract

In Chap. 3 the definition of a general problem is given and different kinds of problems and the concept of problem handling are described. This is followed by a discussion of when is something a problem, for whom it is a problem, a discussion of interventions, who should handle the problem and when a problem is solved. In a brief historical review, the work of a selection of researchers on problem solving in the field of cognitive psychology is discussed. We discussed the differences between the problems investigated by cognitive psychology and complex societal problems. Chapter 3 ended with a definition of complex societal problems.

In this chapter, Human Problem Handling, some similarities between the problems investigated by cognitive psychology and complex societal problems will be discussed. We discuss the process of human problem handling, and we will answer the remaining parts of the research question a3 of expectation a:

what are the similarities relevant for handling complex societal problems between the problems that are studied in the field of Cognitive Psychology and problems in the field of the theory of the methodology of societal complexity (expectation a)?

This chapter begins with a description of the two sub-cycles of the problem handling process in which there are several problem handling phases. The phases of the problem handling process will be described in detail in order to emphasize certain similarities between the problems dealt with by cognitive psychology and complex societal problems. Then there is a discussion on the use of scenarios based on system dynamics models.

The problem handling phases that are identified by other researchers will be discussed, followed by a discussion of rationality in problem handling, and the knowledge and data needed for handling complex societal problems. The discussion in this chapter ends with some remarks about problem handling techniques.

This chapter, Human Problem Handling, is based on Chap. 3 of the dissertation (DeTombe 1994). For Section 4.4.3 material is used from the article

Dorien J. DeTombe & Harm ‘t Hart (1996) Using system dynamic modeling techniques for constructing scenarios of societal problems in Dorien J. DeTombe & Cor VanDijkum (Eds.) Analyzing Societal Problems. Mering: Rainer Hampp Verlag, pp. 57–75. ISBN 3-87988-189-8.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For a description of the concept of the problem owner, see Chap. 7.

  2. 2.

    For the description of the concept of the handling space, see Sect. 4.4.2.

  3. 3.

    As described in Chap. 7, the phases are iteratively traversed several times at different conceptual levels.

  4. 4.

    See Sect. 4.5, for instance, Van Strien (1986).

  5. 5.

    See Chap. 3 Newell and Simon (1972).

  6. 6.

    An example of problem finding where not knowing may be preferable to knowing is having an incurable cancer for which there is no intervention that will diminish the disease. A discussion on this issue is described in the novel De Aansprekers by Maarten ‘t Hart (M. ‘t Hart 1979).

  7. 7.

    The literature discussed in this chapter is mainly taken from 1970s and 1980s. More recent literature shows that these ideas and concepts are still being used. Not much concerning the topic has changed in the meantime.

  8. 8.

    See the definition of problem solving given by Newell and Simon (1972) in Sect. 3.9.4.1.

  9. 9.

    See Sect. 3.9.3.

  10. 10.

    He uses the Dutch word: informatie-versmalling.

  11. 11.

    He uses the Dutch word: informatie-uitbreiding.

  12. 12.

    Since intuition, according to Snoek (1989), occurs more with experienced experts than with students, we believe that what is called intuition is mainly a combination of expert knowledge and experience. However, it is often called intuition because it is hard to reconstruct the thinking process because the thinking process goes very fast. Snoek (1989) considers the use of intuition a very effective and efficient method of thinking. Selz (1922) also refers to intuition. See Sect. 3.9.3.

  13. 13.

    Reflecting on a problem does not always mean that the problem becomes clearer. A problem that looked rather simple at first can become more complicated the more a person reflects on it.

  14. 14.

    Not all problem handling phases are discussed in this chapter in favor of the continuity of the discourse. The phases of putting the problem on the political agenda and deciding to handle the problem (phase 1.3), forming a problem handling team and starting to analyze the problem (phase 1.4), implementing interventions (phase 2.5) and evaluating interventions, and the problem handling process (phase 2.6) will be discussed in Chap. 7, because issues are more related to complex societal problem than with cognitive problems.

  15. 15.

    Also called internal memory or short-term memory

  16. 16.

    Crombag (1984) not only uses the computer as a metaphor for human thinking, but actually compares the human memory with the memory of the computer with regard to direct access of hypotheses and data. In comparison with computer memory, human memory is far more limited in its recall of facts, but much better in combining data and ideas and handle missing parts of data.

  17. 17.

    See the use of enriched pictures as conceptual models of problems in the methodology of soft systems theory of Checkland (1981, 1982, 1990, 1992).

  18. 18.

    Models are used in many disciplines to represent issues.

  19. 19.

    Translation by the author

  20. 20.

    This is important, as we will see later, for making a model of a complex societal problem. The content of the models of the same complex societal problem can vary a great deal. It is difficult to completely determine whether the model is correct, incorrect, or incomplete.

  21. 21.

    Translation by the author

  22. 22.

    When we refer to theories, hypotheses, assumptions, experiences, or intuitions in general, we will use, for the convenience of the reader, the term theoretical ideas.

  23. 23.

    Selecting a theory is more or less arbitrary, depending on a person’s scientific and/or political view and on one’s personal belief, depending on the methodology and the discipline used (Kuhn 1970).

  24. 24.

    See Sect. 4.4.3.

  25. 25.

    See Sect. 4.4.1.2.

  26. 26.

    The mathematical model is often put in formulae.

  27. 27.

    See Chap. 7 the seven-layer communication model.

  28. 28.

    See Chap. 7 the seven-layer communication model.

  29. 29.

    See Sect. 4.4.3.

  30. 30.

    See Sect. 4.4.3.

  31. 31.

    See Fig. 4.7.

  32. 32.

    See Fig. 4.8.

  33. 33.

    See for further discussion on simulation models Sect. 4.4.3 and Sect. 7.5.

  34. 34.

    An often used system dynamic simulation program is the software program Stella.

  35. 35.

    In a model we can call the phenomena and actors variables. A variable gives information about (a part of) a phenomenon and actor involved in the problem.

  36. 36.

    See Sect. 4.4.3.

  37. 37.

    See phases of problem handling in Fig. 4.1.

  38. 38.

    Chaos theory shows that uncertainty is more than a lack of knowledge (VanDijkum and DeTombe 1992b).

  39. 39.

    Cases in the field of a banking system, a library system, and a transport system

  40. 40.

    An entity type is a data category. A person can be an entity type in a database of an organization with the attributes of name, salary, and age.

  41. 41.

    See for an explanation of the seven-layer communication model Chap. 7.

  42. 42.

    For the discussion on climate change, see Chap. 11.

  43. 43.

    See Chap. 11 the discussion on climate change.

  44. 44.

    Examples are the food distribution problem in the world, the quality of water, the refugee problem, and international trade.

  45. 45.

    See Chap. 3.

  46. 46.

    See Chap. 8.

  47. 47.

    See Sect. 4.4.3.1.

  48. 48.

    See Chap. 10 where the focus is on the complex societal problems of credit crisis as a sub-system of the world.

  49. 49.

    See Sect. 4.4.1.

  50. 50.

    One of the reasons for treating a problem suboptimally is that only a part of the problem is handled, instead of the whole problem. Interventions based only on a partial view of the problem can easily lead to pseudosolutions. An example of this is that in the early 1990s, the Ministry of Education in The Netherlands suggested that school career advisers should advise girls to study mathematics and technology instead of, for instance, languages. The advice did not have the positive effect the Ministry had hoped for. One of the reasons for this is that only a small part of the problem was handled, while the main reason, the inferior position of women in society (Beauvoir 1949/1978), was left untouched. In order to handle this problem, the cause and not only one of the symptoms have to be changed (Witte 1994).

  51. 51.

    Interventions are not always needed in order to reach the goal of the problem handling process. Sometimes conclusions can be directly drawn based on the empirical model. In psychology and in psychiatry, the goal of the problem handling is sometimes only to perform a diagnosis.

  52. 52.

    For instance, once a problem is defined as a legal problem, it is implied that a legal practitioner will “solve” the problem. Only occasionally, when new problems arise, there is a consultation of experts from domains other than law. In most of the cases, experts from other fields seldom reflect on the “law” problem again.

  53. 53.

    Translation by the author

  54. 54.

    The Korean War (1950–1953) was about the partition of the Korean country into North Korea and South Korea. The Chinese supported the North Korean side, while the USA and some European troops supported the South Korean side.

  55. 55.

    The Cuban Missile Crisis (October 1962) refers to the situation in the USA after the USA became aware of the action of the USSR of placing missiles in Cuba as a reaction to the failed Pigs Bay invasion of the USA on Cuba earlier that year. This invasion was the result of group think (see Chap. 6). The USSR missiles were meant to defend Cuba from a new invasion by the USA. The missiles were directed to the USA.

  56. 56.

    Translation by the author

  57. 57.

    See also Fig. 4.1.

  58. 58.

    See for further discussion on the desired goal Chap. 6.

  59. 59.

    The central theme of the theater play by George Büchner (1835) “The Death of Danton” is the discussions between Robespierre and Danton about the strategy and means to reach the desired goal of the French revolution (Theater play performed in Amsterdam in 2014 and directed by Johan Simons as Danton’s dood).

  60. 60.

    A homonym is the same word referring to different entities.

  61. 61.

    Redundancy is the same information at more than one place in the information system.

  62. 62.

    An example of this is the fast-changing circumstances in the East European countries in the period of 1989–1991.

  63. 63.

    Such as sensitive military data, data about firms and plants, and politically sensitive data

  64. 64.

    This is often the case with urgent problems as with sudden disasters such as typhoons, airplane crashes, political riots, or coups (Rosenthal 1984).

  65. 65.

    See Sect. 4.4.1.

  66. 66.

    The paradigm of chaos theory focuses on uncertainty in dynamic systems, on unpredictability, and uncertainty in different objects. Chaos theory is a generic term for theoretical ideas and models used by researchers from different fields focusing on the changes in systems where periods of predictable change are alternated by periods of unpredictable change. Order alternates with periods of disorder and chaotic changes. Chaos theory is applied in many disciplines (Broer and Verhulst 1990). See for further discussion on this subject Sect. 4.4.3.1.3.

  67. 67.

    The desired situation can, for instance, be a certain reorganization of an institute or diminishing the discharge of chemical plants.

  68. 68.

    The problem space is the space in which the solution of the problem can be found. We do not use the term “problem space” to avoid the idea that the goal, the operations, and the intermittent actions are already known and that the solution of the problem can be found within the problem space. For complex societal problems, often the desired situation is not clear, or the goals are in conflict with each other, and/or the kind of operations and tools are not clear. This is the reason we avoid the term “problem space.”

  69. 69.

    See Sect. 4.7.

  70. 70.

    An example of the second level of constraints is the start of the European Union in 1992.

  71. 71.

    See, for instance, the situation during the French revolution in 1789 or the evolving situation after the Russian revolution in 1917. At this level, it is also the “dream” of Martin Luther King (1929–1968) to change the society of the USA in order to improve the living conditions for black people, a dream that should be put into actions with many societal changes.

  72. 72.

    “Unfreezing” means inviting people to include, as a higher level of handling space (level three or four), a thought experiment. This can be done in order to stimulate people to think about quite new situations, to realize that the present situation is also constructed by people and as a consequence is not rigidly determined. The hope is that people will come up with quite new and creative ideas for changing the problem.

  73. 73.

    A contemporary example of this is the Woody Allen film “The Purple Rose of Cairo” in which a movie star walks out of the screen and becomes an artificially “real” person (Allen 1985), and another example is the television series Superman, in which a nice, very strong male person, who can fly, is helping other people in an almost supernatural way.

  74. 74.

    This fourth level, the mental realm, can have effects in real life. In Colombia in the nineteenth century, a kind of “Superman,” calling himself Superbario, supported poor people in their struggle to improve their lives. Television commercials also take advantage of the fantasy presenting the idea that using a certain article leads to a desired situation: eating certain slimming products will make you look like the beautiful slim young lady in the picture, and smoking cigarettes makes you a real “he-man.” Also trusting in “God” in case of war “God is on our side,” or in the case of sorrow, might give the person so much self-confidence, which the person feels stronger than usual and more capable to perform his or her tasks.

  75. 75.

    See Chap. 7.

  76. 76.

    Time is an important constraint in urgent problems, like riots and disasters (see Rosenthal 1984).

  77. 77.

    Rijksinstituut voor Volksgezondheid en Milieuhygiëne (a Dutch semigovernmental institute)

  78. 78.

    Nederlands Centrum voor Geestelijke Volksgezondheid (an independent Dutch institute dealing with information, research, and advice in the field of public mental health and public mental healthcare)

  79. 79.

    See also discussion in Sect. 7.4.

  80. 80.

    This criticism comes close to our critique as it is defined in Sect. 4.4.3.1.1 of the Theory of the Methodology of Societal Complexity.

  81. 81.

    See Sect. 4.3.6.

  82. 82.

    For instance, the way institutes such as the National Institute of Public Health and Environmental Protection (RIVM) and the Research Center of Public Mental Health Care (NcGv) make their models

  83. 83.

    See Chap. 7.

  84. 84.

    Here, “it” refers to system dynamic modeling.

  85. 85.

    Chaos refers here to a certain kind of unpredictability. This differs from the commonsense use of the word, which means disordered.

  86. 86.

    Translation by the author

  87. 87.

    See for a historical overview of the development of chaos theory Gleick (1987, pp. 11–54) and Verhulst (1990, pp. 15–33).

  88. 88.

    See for further discussion on this subject Chap. 7.

  89. 89.

    Translation by the author

  90. 90.

    The mathematician Penrose (1989) wonders what the reason is for this indefiniteness of chaotic functions. According to Penrose it can be found in the incompletion theorem of Gödel, which proves that some things are not decidable. The question whether a certain point belongs to the values of a chaotic function or not is not decidable. One can never say with certainty that certain elements do not belong to the chaotic set.

  91. 91.

    For example, the Delphi oracle in Greece in 1400 bc

  92. 92.

    Priest who lived in the period of 500 bc till 0 ad

  93. 93.

    In Western science, predictions are often based on extrapolation or regression analysis. The extrapolation of the future development of phenomena is based on the extrapolation of the development of phenomena. The values range within certain limits (minimum and maximum). In extrapolation the future is predicted on the basis of an analysis of the data of the previous periods in regard to the predicted changes. With regression analysis, future developments are forecast based on the assumption that present causes have future consequences in the same way that past causes have been followed by present consequences.

  94. 94.

    For this we consider that predictions based on system dynamic modeling, if done according to the methodology described in Chap. 7, increase the chance of developing better predictive models for explorations into the future than models based on extrapolation or on regression analysis; however, this statement should be supported based on further scientific research.

  95. 95.

    In making scenarios, one should take some precautions as far as this is possible based on the criticisms formulated on using system dynamic modeling for future exploration as seen in this chapter.

  96. 96.

    A null-option is that no explicit elaborate interventions are made. The situations will remain the same as they always have been (Bots et al. 1992). Scenarios based on the null-option can also be used to explore the relation between the variables in the model (VanDijkum 1992).

  97. 97.

    See Sect. 4.7.1.

  98. 98.

    Even small changes in a societal equilibrium can encounter much resistance. The introduction of societal change or changes within organizations should be carefully prepared and guided before they can be implemented. As is described in Chap. 7 about the Compram methodology, the societal reactions should be anticipated before the implementation of the proposed interventions.

  99. 99.

    An example of this is curing leprosy throughout the world. Knowing how to cure the disease is different from actually solving the leprosy problem.

  100. 100.

    Especially with productive ways of thinking, there is a distinction between discovering the problem handling method that can be applied and the actual application itself.

  101. 101.

    This was often the case at the beginning of computer controlled automation in the 1980s and 1990s of the twentieth century. After starting with problem analysis and spending 2 years on building a computer system to solve the problem, people realized that the new system no longer fitted the contemporary situation at the moment of its implementation.

  102. 102.

    See Chap. 2.

  103. 103.

    Translation by the author

  104. 104.

    One tries something and sees the effect, and when the effect does not meet the desired situation, one goes back and reconsiders the problem, this time with the new knowledge.

  105. 105.

    As we will state in Chap. 7, one should never skip a problem handling phase.

  106. 106.

    Translation by the author

  107. 107.

    See our comment earlier in this chapter. However not all the phases have to be worked through; one can also stop and leave the problem as it is.

  108. 108.

    As discussed in Chap. 7, the problem definition of the client, the problem owner, is in the case of a complex societal problem often only a part of the whole problem. After analyzing the problem, the problem looks often different.

  109. 109.

    For instance, in problem handling phase 2.1, in developing the empirical model, it can happen that one realizes that there are relations between the variables other than was previously supposed. In that case, it is sensible to return to problem handling phase 1.5 and reformulate the hypotheses about the relations between the phenomena and actors.

  110. 110.

    See also Sect. 4.7.3 and the discussion in Sect. 4.3.5 on hypotheses.

  111. 111.

    Intuition and experience also play a role in the description of the problem handling activities of neurology experts [see Sect. 3.9.1, Snoek (1989)].

  112. 112.

    First published by Simon in 1945

  113. 113.

    See Sect. 4.4.2.

  114. 114.

    The coincidental connection between action and positive result can be strengthened by reinforcement.

  115. 115.

    Trial and error is a commonly used research technique in medical research and in chemical research in order to find answers to problems.

  116. 116.

    See Sect. 4.6.1.

  117. 117.

    This still is the leading opinion. Although chaos theory shows that even when an algorithm is defined, in some cases there cannot be an unambiguous solution.

  118. 118.

    See the theories of decision making in Sect. 4.6.1.

  119. 119.

    See also the phases of Van Strien in Sect. 4.5.

  120. 120.

    See Sect. 3.9.6.

  121. 121.

    See Sect. 4.6.1.

  122. 122.

    More heuristics will follow in Chap. 7.

  123. 123.

    See Sect. 3.2.

  124. 124.

    We are aware that the concept “normally educated” is vague and tendentious. By “normally educated,” we mean what can be expected in relation to the function, the level of education, and the age of a child or an adult according to his or her culture at a certain time.

  125. 125.

    An example of the misconception that a situation remains constant can be seen in the early days of automation. A frequent mistake in building a large database for an organization was to assume that the organizations would not change during the 1–2 years of development. However, by the time the database was finished, the organization had changed so much that the information in the database did not meet contemporary needs (see the discussion in Sect. 4.4.5).

References

  • Abraham, F. D. (1995). Dynamics, bifurcation, self-organization, chaos, mind, conflict, insensitivity, initial conditions, time, unification, diversity, free will and social responsibility. In R. Robertson & A. Combs (Eds.), Chaos theory in psychology and the life sciences. Hove, UK: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackoff, R. L. (1962). Scientific method: Optimizing applied research decisions. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackoff, R. L. (1974). Redesigning the future. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackoff, R. L. (1978). The art of problem solving. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackoff, R. L. (1981). Creating the corporative future. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackoff, R. L. (1999). Ackoff’s best: His classic writings on management. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackoff, R. L. (2001). OR: After the post mortem. System Dynamics review, 17(4), 341–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, W. (1985). The purple rose of Cairo. Motion picture. Los Angeles, CA: Orion Pictures Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apostel, L. (1960). Towards the formal study of models. Synthèse, XII(2–3), 125–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachrach, P., & Baratz, M. S. (1963). Decisions and nondecisions. American Political Science Review, 57, 632–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P., & Flora, J. (1977). Semantic and perceptual processes in symbolic comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 1(2), 278–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barre, D., de Graaf, R., & Voragen, F. (1982). De Huisvesting van Studerenden in Amsterdam. Amsterdam. Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and nature: A necessary unity (Advances in Systems Theory, Complexity, and the Human Sciences). New York, NY: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baybrooke, D., & Lindblom, C. E. (1963). A strategy of decision. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauvoir, S. (1949/1978). De tweede sexe. Utrecht: Erven J. Bijleveld. (Le Deuxième sexe. Paris: Edition Gallimard).

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, J. B., Turner, T. J., & Bower, G. H. (1979). Point of view in narrative comprehension, memory, and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 309–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobrow, D. G., & Norman, D. A. (1975). Some principles of memory schemata. In D. G. Bobrow & A. Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding. New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boden, M. A. (1988). Computer models of mind: Computational approaches in theoretical psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boden, M. A. (1990). The creative mind: Myth and mechanisms. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boonman, J. H., & Kok, W. A. M. (1986). Kennis verwerven uit studieteksten. Utrecht: Vakgroep Onderwijskunde Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bots, P. W. G. (1989). An environment to support problem solving. Delft: Delft University of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bots, P. W. G., Sol, H. G., & Thissen, W. A. H. (1992). Handout college Inleiding Technische Bestuurskunde (TB 1010). Delft: TU Delft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bree, D. S. (red.) (1989). Vuistregels bij besluitvorming. Rotterdam: Universitaire pers Rotterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F. (1987). Schemas versus mental models in human memory. In P. Morris (Ed.), Modelling cognition. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broer, H. W., & Verhulst, F. (red.) (1990). Dynamische systemen en chaos. Utrecht: Epsilon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brookshear, J. G. (1991). Computer science. An overview. The Benjamin/Cummings Publishing Company Inc. San Francisco (third edition).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruckmann, G., & Fleissner, P. (1989). Am Steuerrad der Wirtschaft. Ein ökonomisch-kybernetisches Modell für Österreich. Wien: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brugman, G. M. (1991). Problem findings: Discovering and formatting problems. European Journal for High Ability, 2(2), 212–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given. In H. Guber (Ed.), Contemporary approaches to cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Büchner, G. (1835). Dantons Tod. Frankfurt am Main: Phönix, Frühlings-Zeitung für Deutschland (journal); Phönix-Verlag (Johann David Sauerländer Verlag) (book).

    Google Scholar 

  • Checkland, P. B. (1981). Systems thinking, systems practice. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Checkland, P. B. (1992). Soft systems methodology in action. Bulletin of the Institute of Management Sciences, 22(1), 181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claparède, E. (1933). La génèse de l’hypothèse. Archive de Psychologie (Vol. 24, pp. 92–94). Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Codd, E. F. (1990). The relational model for database management. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cofer, C. N., Chmielewksi, D. L., & Brockway, J. F. (1976). Constructive processes and the structure of human memory. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), The structure of human memory. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crombag, H. F. M. (1978). Over het oplossen van meetkundige bewijsopgaven. Nederlands tijdschrift voor psychologie en haar grensgebieden, 33(2), 105–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crombag, H. F. M. (1984). Slecht gedefinieerde problemen. Kennis, mens en computer/2. Intermediair, 20, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crombag, H. F. M., de Wijkerslooth, J. L., & Cohen, M. J. (1977). Een theorie over rechtelijke beslissingen. Groningen: Tjeenk Willink.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daft, R. L. (1986). Organisation theory and design. St. Paul: West Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groot de, A. D. (1946/1965). Thought and choice in chess. New York, NY: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groot de, A. D. (1969). Methodology. Foundations of inference and research in the behavioral sciences. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J. (1992a). Manager training with cases. In L. Piecuch (Ed.), Symulacyjne Modele Przedsiebiorstw (pp. 192–208). Krakow: Akademii Ekonomicznej w Krakowie, Polska.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J. (1992b). Onzekerheden in de voorspelling van de Aids epidemie. In C. VanDijkum & D. de Tombe (Eds.), Gamma Chaos. Onzekerheid en orde in de menswetenschappen (pp. 126–137). Bloemendaal: Aramith Uitgevers (published as Tombe de, D. J.). ISBN 90-6834-105-7.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J. (1992c). Using a simulation tool for defining global problems. ISAGA’91; 22nd Annual International Conference of the International Simulation and Gaming Association, Kyoto, Japan (Abstract).

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J. (1993h). Cooperative defining of complex societal problems The AIDS problem. In R. Glanville & G. de Zeeuw (Eds.), Problems of support, survival and culture (pp. 305–319). Amsterdam: Thesis Publishers (published as DeTombe, D. J.).

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J. (1994). Defining Complex Interdisciplinary Societal Problems. A Theoretical Study for Constructing a Cooperative Problem Analyzing Method: The Method COMPRAM. Amsterdam: Thesis Publishers, ISBN 90 5170 302-3.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J. (1996e). Een mogelijke bijdrage van socio-cybernetica aan het analyseren, sturen en evalueren van complexe technisch bestuurskundige problemen. Een discussie in Wetenschappelijk Tijdschift voor Modelbouw en Informatica (C. vanDijkum & D. DeTombe (Eds.)) 96, 1–4; 97, 1–2.

  • DeTombe, D. J. (2000f). Non-linear moments in technological development. Some methodological issues. In E. A. Stuhler & M. Vezjak (Eds.), Sustainable development: The role of the Universities. Research on cases and theories (Vol. 7, pp. 79–91). Munchen/Mering: Hampp. ISSN 0940-2829.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J., & Hart, ‘t, H. (1996). Using system dynamic modeling techniques for constructing scenarios of societal problems. In D. J. DeTombe & C. vanDijkum (Eds.), Analyzing societal problems (pp. 57–75). Rainer Hampp: Mering. ISBN 3-87988-189-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeTombe, D. J., & VanDijkum, C. (1993). Non-linear moments in technological development. In L. Leydesdorff (Ed.), New developments in technology studies: Evolutionary economics and chaos theory (pp. 30–32). Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1933). How we think. New York, NY: Heath.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, J. T. (1988). Levels of problem finding versus problem solving. Questioning Exchange, 2, 105–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doerbecker, C. (1979). Het iteratieve zoekproces en het cognitieve kader van de cliënt. Tijdschrift voor Agologie, 9, 355–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. M., & Stea, D. (Eds.). (1973). Image and environment. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dror, Y. (1964). Muddling through: ‘Science’ of inertia? Public Administration Review, 24, 153–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elstein, A. S., Schulman, L. S., & Sprafka, S. A. (1978). Medical problem solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etzioni, A. (1968). The active Society: A theory of societal and political processes. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flood, R. L., & Jackson, M. C. (1991). Creative problem solving. Total systems intervention. Chicester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrester, J. W. (1961). Industrial dynamics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrester, J. W. (1963). Urban dynamics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrester, J. W. (1987). Lessons from system dynamics modeling. System Dynamics Review, 3(2), 136–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). De emoties. Een overzicht van onderzoek en theorie. Amsterdam: Bert Bakker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1984). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. London: Heineman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1977). How perception really develops: A view from outside the network. In D. Laberge & S. J. Samuals (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, G. (1960). System simulation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. (1972). Knowledge and human interests. London: Heineman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, ‘t M. (1979). De aansprekers. Amsterdam: De Arbeiderspers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, ‘t H. (1991). Onderzoek en Praktijkfasen. Vakgroep Ontwikkeling en Socialisatie, SektieMethoden, Utrecht. In J. van Dijk, M. de Goede, H. Hart ‘t, & J. Teunissen (Eds.), Onderzoeken en veranderen: Methoden van praktijkonderzoek. Leiden: Stenfert Kroese.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Ellis, M. C. (1979). Constructive memory for bizarre and sensible sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Learning and Memory, 5(4), 386–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, M. C., & Miller, J. G. (2000). Peter Checkland at 70: A review of soft system thinking. Systems Research and Behavioral Science, 17(S1), 1007–1011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jager, J. C., Postma, M. J., Leidl, R., Majnoni d’Intignano, B., & Baert, A. E. (1990). AIDS impact scenarios: Questions for the years to come. AIDS, 1990(4), 1166–1167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1980). Mental models in cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 4, 71–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1988). The computer and the mind. London: Fontana Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorgenson, D. W. (1978). The role of energy in the US economy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Institute of Economic Research, Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1976). Memory for prose. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), The structure of the human memory. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85(5), 363–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klabbers, J. H. G. (1989). Methodologische aspecten van het ontwerpen van leeromgevingen. In J. Klep & P. Kommers (Eds.), Courseware en leerplanontwikkeling, didactische systeemanalyse, studies in leerplanontwikkeling (Vol. 14). Enschede: SLO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laplace, D. S. (1796). Exposition du Systeme du Monde. Paris: Fayard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The biological foundations of language. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leydesdorff, L., Besselaar, van den, P., Allen, P. M., DeTombe, D., Nelson, R. R., & Rip, A. (1994). Evolutionary economics and chaos theory: New directions in technology studies. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: St. Martin’s Press/Palgrave Macmillan. ISBN 0312122187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, C. E. (1959). The science of muddling through. Public Administration Review, 19(1), 79.

    Google Scholar 

  • March, J. G. (1978, Autumn). Bounded rationality, ambiguity, and the engineering of choice. The Bell Journal of Economics, 9(2), 587–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. (1977). Self schemata and processing of information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 63–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, K. (1890). Das Kapital. Kritik der politischen Oekonomie. Hamburg: Otto Meissner.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1970). The acquisition of languages; the study of development psycholinguistics. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meadows, D. (1980). The unavoidable a priori. In J. Randers (Ed.), Elements of the system dynamics method. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meadows, D. H., Meadows, D. L., Randers, R., & Behrens, W. W., III. (1972). The limits to growth. London: Potomac Associates Book/Earth Island.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meadows, D. H., Meadows, D. L., & Randers, J. (1992). Beyond the limits. Global collapse or a sustainable future. London: Earth Scan Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. W., & Starr, M. K. (1960). Executive decisions and operations research. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. In P. H. Winston (Ed.), The psychology of computer vision. New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, J. R. (1971). Aida a technique for the management of design. Coventry: Tavistock Institute of Human Relations. Institute of Operational Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mynatt, C. R., Doherty, M. E., & Tweney, R. D. (1977). Confirmation bias in a simulation research environment: An experimental study for scientific inference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 85–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality; principles and implications of cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1969). Memory and attention. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1976). On the role of the active memory processes in perception and cognition. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), The structure of human memory. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.). (1975). Exploration in cognition. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivo, A. (1969, May). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychological Review, 76, 241–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penrose, R. (1989). The emperor’s new mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. N. (1981). The mind’s best work. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polya, G. (1957). How to solve it (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Doubleday Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of Chaos. New York, NY: Bantham Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahmandad, H., & Sterman, J. (2005). Heterogeneity and network structure in the dynamics of diffusion: Comparing agent-based and differential equation models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskey, D. R. (1979). Verbal memory process in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Learning and Memory, 5, 271–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, U. (1984). Rampen, rellen, gijzelingen. Amsterdam: De Bataafsche Leeuw.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossman, J. (1931). The psychology of the inventor. A study of the patentee. Washington, DC: The Inventor’s Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau, J. J. (1762). Emile ou de l’education. Paris: Garnier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In D. Laberge & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Basic processes in reading; perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1984). Schemata and the cognitive system. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 161–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & Norman, D. A. (1978). Accretion, tuning and restructuring: Three modes of learning. In J. W. Cotton & R. Klatzky (Eds.), Semantic factors in cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. T. (1967). De psychologie van de informatieverwerking. Arnhem: Van Loghum Slaterus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schein, E. H. (1969). Process consultation: Its role in organizational development. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selz, O. (1922). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens und des Irrtums. Eine experimentalle Untersuchung. Bonn: Cohen. Some parts in English translation in Frijda, N. H., Groot de, A. D. (Eds.) (1981). Otto Selz: His contribution to psychology (pp. 76–146). Den Haag: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sévon, G. (1984). Cognitive maps of past and future economic events. In K. B. Borcherding, C. V. Brehmer, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.), Research perspectives on decision making under uncertainty. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1957). Administrative behavior: A study of decision making processes in administrative organization. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1969). The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1979). Models of thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair-de Zwart, H. (1973). Language acquisition and cognitive development. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behaviour. New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snoek, J. W. (1989). Het denken van de neuroloog. Groningen: Dijkhuizen van Zanten.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sol, H. G. (1982). Simulation in information systems development. Groningen: Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spilich, G. J., Vesonder, G. T., & Chiesi, H. L. (1979). Text processing of domain related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 18, 275–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J. (1977). Remembering information from text: The ‘state of schema’ approach. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L. (1987). The deepening of expert systems. AI Communications, 1, 9–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Smith, E. E. (Eds.). (1988). The psychology of human thought. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarski, A. (1953). Inleiding tot de logica en tot de methodenleer der deductieve wetenschappen (Introduction to logic and to the methodology of deductive sciences). Amsterdam: N.V. Noord-Hollandsche Uitgevers Maatschappij.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennekes, H. (1990a). Dan leef ik liever in onzekerheid. Bloemendaal: Aramith.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennekes, H. (Ed.). (1990b). De vlinder van Lorenz. De verrassende dynamica van chaos. Bloemendaal: Aramith.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoreau, H. D. (1854). Walden; or, life in the woods. Boston, MA: Ticknor and Fields.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1979). The use of schemata in the acquisition and transfer of knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 11(1), 82–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toffler, A. (1987). De derde golf. Utrecht: Veen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsujimoto, R. N., Wilde, J., & Robertson, D. R. (1978, December). Distorted memory for exemplars of a social structure: Evidence for schematic memory processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(12), 1402–1414.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanDijkum, C. (1992). Module model – en data theorie. Lectures for social science students. Utrecht: Utrecht University.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanDijkum, C., & DeTombe, D. J. (Eds.). (1992b). Gamma Chaos. Onzekerheid en orde in de menswetenschappen. Bloemendaal: Aramith Uitgevers (published as de Tombe). ISBN 90-68341057.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanDijkum, C., & DeTombe, D. (1992c). Inleiding. In C. vanDijkum & D. de Tombe (Eds.), Gamma Chaos. Onzekerheid en orde in de menswetenschappen (pp. 7–9). Bloemendaal: Aramith Uitgevers (published as de Tombe). ISBN 90-6834-105-7.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Duyne, P. C. (1983). Beslissen in eenvoud. Hoe officieren van justitie over strafzaken beslissen. Arnhem: Gouda Quint.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Strien, P. J. (1975). Psychologie in 1975: theorie en praktijk van een veranderende wetenschap. Opgedragen aan Prof. dr. J. Th. Snijders. Groningen: Tjeenk Willink.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Strien, P. J. (1986). Praktijk als wetenschap. Methodologie van het sociaal-wetenschappelijk handelen. Maastricht: Van Gorkum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhulst, F. (1990). De historische route naar chaos. In H. Tennekens (Ed.), De vlinder van Lorenz. De verrassende dynamica van chaos. Bloemendaal: Aramith Uitgevers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vlek, C. A. J., & Wagenaar, W. A. (1979). Judgment and decision under uncertainty. In A. Michon, E. G. Eijkman, & L. F. W. de Klerk (Eds.), Handbook of psychonomics (Vol. II, pp. 253–345). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz, H. L. F. (1867). Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. Leipzig: Leopold Voss.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagenaar, W. A., & Loftus, E. F. (1991). Tien identificaties door ooggetuigen: logische en procedurele problemen. In P. J. van Koppen & H. T. M. Crombag (Eds.), De menselijke factor. Psychologie voor juristen (pp. 175–208). Arnhem: Gouda Quint BV.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierda, F. W. (1991). Developing interorganizational information systems. Delft: Wierda.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witte, M. (1994). Meisjes meegerekend. De constructie van wiskundige begaafdheid. Amsterdam: Thesis Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wortman, P. M. (1966). Representation and strategy in diagnostic problem solving. Human Factors, 8, 45–53 (50: 48-53).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

DeTombe, D. (2015). Human Problem Handling. In: Handling Societal Complexity. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-43917-3_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics