Echolocation in Whirligig Beetles Using Surface Waves: An Unsubstantiated Conjecture

Chapter
Part of the Animal Signals and Communication book series (ANISIGCOM, volume 3)

Abstract

Understanding the physics of signal transmission in insect vibratory communication is nearly impossible for most natural substrates on which insect communicates, except for the water surface, on which the propagation of linear capillary surface waves is reasonably well mastered. The water surface is an environment where several insect species live and exploit its static and dynamic deformations. Whirligig beetles use surface waves in a wide range of situations, and it has been hypothesized that they could use the echo of their own waves to scan the water surface. These insects were also suspected to perceive menisci, i.e., static deformations of the water surface. However, no manipulative experimental studies, based on quantitative predictions of the surface deformation, have been conducted to determine whether echolocation or meniscus perception is used. The aim of this work was to test the hypothesis that whirligig beetles detect the meniscus around immobile objects. If the perception of an immobile object on the water surface can be explained by resorting to the meniscus only, then the hypothesis of echolocation should be reconsidered. We used cylinders of varying diameter and wettability crossing the water surface to experimentally modify the meniscus profiles. Contacts between beetles and cylinders were recorded using a high-speed camera. Loops in trajectories before or after a contact, as well as unfolding of forelegs before a contact, were used as criteria for the distance at which cylinders were perceived. Based on a theoretical modeling of the meniscus profiles, we predicted the distance at which one type of cylinder was detected based on the meniscus amplitude corresponding to the distance of perception of another type of cylinder. Both diameter size and wettability affected cylinder perception. Our predictions were unfortunately contradictory, and the unfolding of forelegs could not be explained by meniscus perception only. Complementary experiments are needed to eliminate any influence of vision. We obtained conflicting evidence about meniscus perception, with the statistically most powerful of our tests being strongly in favor of it. Thus, echolocation by whirligig beetles should still be considered as a conjecture, at best.

Keywords

Dust Assure Polystyrene Fishing Arena 

References

  1. Bennett RR (1967) Spectral sensitivity studies on whirligig beetles, Dineutes ciliatus. J Insect Physiol 13:621–626, IN9, 627–633Google Scholar
  2. Bleckmann H, Borchardt M, Horn P, Gorner P (1994) Stimulus discrimination and wave source localization in fishing spiders (Dolomedes triton and D. okefinokensis). J Comp Physiol A 174:305–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bleckmann H, Topp G (1981) Surface wave sensitivity of the lateral line organs of the topminnow Aplocheilus lineatus. Naturwissenschaften 68:624–625CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bott RH (1928) Beitrage zur kenntnis von Gyrinus natator substriatus Steph. Z Morphol Oekol Tiere 10:207–306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carthy JD, Goodman LJ (1964) An electrophysiological investigation of the divided eye of Gyrinus bicolor. J Insect Physiol 10:431–436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Claas B, Munz H, Gorner P (1993) Reaction to surface waves by Xenopus laevis Daudin. Are sensory systems other than the lateral line involved? J Comp Physiol A 172:759–765PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. de Gennes PG, Brochard-Wyart F, Quéré D (2003) Capillary and wetting phenomena: drops, bubbles, pearls and waves. Springer, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  8. Dean RG, Dalrymple RA (1991) Water wave mechanics for engineers and scientists. World Scientific, SingaporeGoogle Scholar
  9. Denny MW (1993) Air & water: the biology and physics of life’s media. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  10. Dusenbery DB (1992) Sensory ecology: how organisms acquire and respond to information. Freeman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  11. Eggers F (1926) The presumed function of Johnston’s sensory organ in Gyrinus. Zool Anz 68:184–192Google Scholar
  12. Eggers F (1927) Further information on the Johnston’s sensory organ and avoidance ability of whirligig beetles. Zool Anz 71:136–156Google Scholar
  13. Hill PSM (2008) Vibrational communication in animals. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  14. Hu DL, Bush JWM (2005) Meniscus-climbing insects. Nature 437:733–736PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. James DF (1974) The meniscus on the outside of a small circular cylinder. J Fluid Mech 63:657–664CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kolmes SA (1983) Ecological and sensory aspects of prey capture by the whirligig beetle Dineutus discolor (Coleoptera, Gyrinidae). J New York Entomol S 91:405–412Google Scholar
  17. Lang HH (1980) Surface-wave discrimination between prey and nonprey by the back swimmer Notonecta glauca L (Hemiptera, Heteroptera). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 6:233–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Le Merrer M, Clanet C, Quéré D, Raphaël E, Chevy F (2011) Wave drag on floating bodies. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108:15064–15068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lighthill J (1978) Waves in fluids. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  20. Murphey RK (1971) Sensory aspects of control of orientation to prey by the waterstrider, Gerris reminis. Z vergl Physiol 72:168–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Rudolph P (1967) Zum Ortungsverfahren von Gyrinus substriatus Steph. Z vergl Physiol 56:341–375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Tucker VA (1969) Wave-making by whirligig beetles (Gyrinidae). Science 166:897–899PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Vogel S (2003) Comparative biomechanics: life’s physical world. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  24. Voise J, Casas J (2010) The management of fluid and wave resistances by whirligig beetles. J Roy Soc Interface 7:343–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Voise J, Schindler M, Casas J, Raphaël E (2011) Capillary-based static self-assembly in higher organisms. J Roy Soc Interface 8:1357–1366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Wiese K (1974) The mechanoreceptive system of prey localization in Notonecta II. The principle of prey localization. J Comp Physiol 92:317–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Wilcox RS (1972) Communication by surface waves: mating behavior of a water strider (Gerridae). J Comp Physiol 80:255–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Wilde JD (1941) Contribution to the physiology of the Johnston organ and its part in the behavior of the Gyrinus. Arch Neer de Physiol 25:381–400Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Tours, IRBI UMR CNRS 6035ToursFrance

Personalised recommendations