Skip to main content

Abstract

The search for the meaning of meaning has been pursued for hundreds of years by philosophers and theologians, but until recently only sporadic forays into this misty territory have been attempted by psychologists. It is understandable that this unmapped quagmire has been avoided, and that some hardy adventurers who set out to explore it bogged down. The difficulty of the problems can be appreciated by trying to talk about the meaning of meaning without using the word itself or one of its close synonyms. Yet, according to a standard word count,1 the word “mean” is one of the commonest words in the language, and the concept is so taken for granted that we never wonder if we know what we mean when we say, “I mean.” However, as Brodbeck (1963) pointed out, meaning is a term within science, and as such it must itself be given meaning. That this has not yet been accomplished is evident not only from the statements made by psychologists and philosophers, but also from the nature of the attempts to define the term in the psychologists’ experimental laboratories and from the theoretical controversies which have arisen from those attempts.

If the translation out of common sense into science is to be made at all, psychology is the science in which the equivalent of meaning will be found [Titchener (1915) p. 118].

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Brodbeck, M. Meaning and action. Phil. Sci., 1963, 30(4), 309–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchler, J. (Ed.). Philosophical Writings of Peirce. New York: Dover, 1955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnap, R. Introduction to Semantics. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univer. Press, 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnap, R. Meaning and Necessity. Chicago: Univer. of Chicago Press, 1947.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (Ed.). Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. New York: John Wiley, and Sons, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambliss, R. Mead’s way out of the basic dilemma in modern existential thought. J. soc. Psychol., 1963, 60(2), 213–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton, and Co., 1957. Review of Verbal Behavior, By Skinner, B. F., Language, 1959, 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Association by contiguity. In: Koch, S. II. 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. (Ed.). Psychology: A Study of a Science. II. General systematic formulations, learning, and special processes. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. (Ed.). Psychology: A Study of a Science. VI. Investigations of man as socius: Their place in psychology and the social sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, S. Philosophy in a New Key. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univer. Press, 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, S. Philosophical Sketches. New York: Mentor Books, New Amer. Libr. of World Lit., 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. Mind, Self, and Society. Chicago: Univer. of Chicago Press, 1934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. The Philosophy of the Act. Chicago: Univer. of Chicago Press, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. Social Psychology. In: Strauss, A. 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, C. W. Signs, Language and Behavior. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogden, C. K.,, and Richards, I. A. The Meaning of Meaning. London: Rout-ledge, and Kegan, Paul, Ltd., 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oscood, C. E. Psycholinguistics. In: KocH, S. VI. 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. Philosophical writings. In: Buchler, J. 1955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. An Inquiry into Meaning and Truth. New York: Norton, 1940.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. Dictionary of Mind, Matter, and Morals. New York: Philosophical Library, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, A. (Ed.). George Herbert Mead on Social Psychology. Chicago: Univer. of Chicago Press, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L.,, and Lorge, I. The Teacher’s Word Book of 30,000 Words. New York: Teacher’s College ( Bureau of Publications ), Columbia Univer., 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. A Beginner’s Psychology. New York: MacMillan, 1915.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A behavioristic theory of ideas. Psychol. Rev., 1926, 33, 352–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men. New York: Century, 1932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H.,, and Kaplan, B. Symbol Formation. New York: John Wiley, and Sons, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. Language, thought and reality. In: Carroll, J. B. 1956.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1966 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Creelman, M.B. (1966). General Orientation. In: The Experimental Investigation of Meaning. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-40332-7_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-40332-7_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-39296-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-40332-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics