Skip to main content

The Bilateral Cooperative Model of Reading

  • Chapter
The Alphabet and the Brain

Abstract

The Bilateral Cooperative Model of reading (BLC model) provides a descriptive framework within which not only reading, but also many of the processes used in language and symbolic thought may be considered. It is not a mathematical model of cognitive processes, and may not be easily amenable to computer simulation. Nevertheless, its use has proved stimulating to the author in considering many issues of perception, cognition, and scientific thought. In this chapter, the BLC model is described and its descriptive value illustrated through examples from the experimental psychology literature, dealing with levels of abstraction from recognizing letters to learning complex cognitive structures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1979). Models of word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 133–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alegria, J., Pignot, E., and Morais, J. (1982). Phonetic analysis of speech and memory codes in beginning readers. Memory and Cognition, 10, 451–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amano, K. (1970). Formation of the act of analyzing phonemic structure of words and its relation to learning Japanese syllabic characters (Kanamoji). Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 1970, 18, 76 — 89 (in Japanese with English abstract).

    Google Scholar 

  • Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J., and Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific mechanisms in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 386 — 393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, J. (1973). Similarity grouping of curves. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 1331–1341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, J. (1983). Textural segmentation, second-order statistics, and textural elements. Biological Cybernetics 48, 125 —130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, H. (1971). Visual recognition of isolated lower-case letters. Vision Research, 11, 459 — 474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E., and Broadbent, M. H. P. (1980). Priming and the active/passive model of word recognition. In Nickerson, R. S. (Ed), Attention and performance, vol 8. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, H.H., Michel, D., Powelson, J., and Gardner, H. (1983). Surprise but coherence: Sensitivity to verbal humor in right-hemisphere patients. Brain and Language, 18, 20–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, B. E., and Hains, S. (1979). Individual differences in word recognition latency. Memory and Cognition, 7, 68–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capra, F. (1976). The Tao of physics. Boulder: Stromhala.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, P. A., and Just, M.A. (1975). Sentence comprehension: a psycholinguistic processing model of verification. Psychological Review, 82, 45–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheeseman, J., and Merikle, P. (1985). Word recognition and consciousness. In Besner, D., Waller, T. G., and MacKinnon, G. E. (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, Vol. S. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chu-Chang, M., and Loritz, D.J. (1977). Phonological encoding of Chinese ideograms in short-term memory. Language Learning, 27, 344–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. H., and Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 472–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, P. (1968). Word association. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groot, A. M. B. (1983). The range of automatic spreading activation in word priming. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 22, 417–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groot, A.M. B., Thomassen, A. J. W. M., and Hudson, P. T. W. (1982). Associative facilitation of word recognition as measured from a neutral prime. Memory and Cognition, 10, 358–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, P., and Becker, C.A. (1982). Semantic context effects in visual word recognition, sentence processing, and reading: Evidence for semantic strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 739–756.

    Google Scholar 

  • Entwisle, D. R., Forsyth, D. F., and Muus, R. (1964). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift in children’s word association. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 19–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ervin, S.M. (1961). Changes with age in verbal determinants of word association. American Journal of Psychology, 74, 361–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. B. (1981). Visual word recognition in Serbo-Croatian is necessarily phonological.

    Google Scholar 

  • Status report on speech research SR-66. Haskins Laboratories, April-June.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foss, D.J. (1982). A discourse on semantic priming. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 590–607.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B. (1980). Identity without form: abstract representations of letters. Perception and Psychophysics 28, 53–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gekoski, W.L., Jacobson, J. Z., and Frazao-Brown, A. P. (1982). Visual masking and linguistic independence in bilinguals. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 108–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. M., and Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V.L., and Bellugi, U. (1982). On the role of sign order and morphological structure in memory for American Sign Language sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 621–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, G. (1983). Semantic interpretation against ambiguity. Technical report CS-83–25. Providence, RI: Brown University Dept of Computer Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: a survey and appraisal. The Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holley-Wilcox, P., and Blank, M.A. (1980). Evidence for multiple access in the processing of isolated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 75–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, V. M. (1979). Accessing ambiguous words during sentence comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 569–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isenor, D. K., and Zaky, S. G. (1986). Fingerprint identification using graph matching. Pattern Recognition, 19, 113–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, F., and Monod, J. (1961). On the regulation of gene activity. In Cellular regulatory mechanisms, vol 26. Cold Spring Harbour symposia on quantitative biology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, J. Z. (1974). Interaction of similarity to words of visual masks and targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 431–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, J. Z. (1976) Visual masking by homonyms. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 30, 174–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, J.Z., and Rhinelander, G. (1978). Gemoetric and semantic similarity in visual masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 224–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, H. (1970) Sign, aymbol and script. London: Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. C., and McClelland, J. L. (1973). Visual factors in word perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 365–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. C., and McClelland, J. L. (1980). Experimental tests of a hierarchical model of word identification. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 503–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. (1982). The integrative action of the cerebral hemispheres. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 423–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. ( 1981 a). A theory of preattentive texture discrimination based on first-order statistics of textons. Biological Cybernetica, 41, 131–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. ( 1981 b). Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions. Nature, 290, 91–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohonen, T. (1982). Self-organized formation of topologically correct feature maps. Biological Cybernetics, 43, 59–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S.A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S.A. (1982). The lateral distribution of event-related potentials during sentence processing.Neuropsychologia, 20, 579–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., Hunt, E. B., and Mathews, N. (1978). Individual differences in the verification of sentence-picture relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 493–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. J. (1980). Conscious and preconscious recognition of polysemous words: locating the selective effects of prior verbal context. In Nickerson, R. S. (Ed.), Attention and Performance, vol 8. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, S. M. (1981). ERIS-context-sensitive coding in speech perception. Journal of Phonetics, 9, 197–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer, E. S. (1980). Remarks on ancient Egyptian writing with emphasis on its mnemonic aspects. In Kolers, R. A., Wrolstad, M. E., and Bouma, H. (Eds). Processing of visible language, vol. 2. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., and Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muraishi, S., and Amano, K. (1972). Reading and writing abilities of preschoolers: a summary. Tokyo: The National Language Research Institute (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishikawa, Y., and Niina, S. (1981). Modes of information processing underlying hemispheric functional differences. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 51, 335–341 (in Japanese with English abstract).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogbom, J. M., and Johnson, L. (1982). Conversation Theory. Technical Report MCSG/TR30. Div. of Cybernetics, Brunel University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pask, G. (1984). Review of Conversation Theory and a protologic (or protolanguage) Lp. Education Communications and Technology Journal, 32, 3–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pask, G., and Scott, B.C. E. (1972). Learning strategies and individual competence. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 4, 217–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pask, G., and Scott, B. C. E. (1973). CASTE: a system for exhibiting learning strategies and regulating uncertainties. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 5, 17–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petrey, S. (1977). Word associations and the development of lexical memory. Cognition, 5, 57–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rausch, R. (1981). Lateralization of temporal lobe dysfunction and verbal encoding. Brain and Language, 12, 92–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., and Posnansky, C. (1978). Stages of processing in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 64–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., and Norman, D.A. (1973). Active semantic networks as a model of human memory. Report CHIP 33. Center for Human Information Processing. San Diego: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., Lindsay, P. H., and Norman, D.A. (1972). A process model for long-term memory. In Tulving, E., and Donaldson, W. (Eds.), Organization of memory. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1972). Conceptual dependency: a theory of natural language understanding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 552–631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schvaneveldt, R. W., Meyer, D. E., and Becker, C.A. (1976). Lexical ambiguity, semantic context, and visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 243–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scribner, S., and Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selfridge, O. (1959). Pandemonium: a paradigm for learning. In Symposium on the mechanisation of thought processes. London: HM Stationery Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, W.J. (1973). Verb types in modern English. Language Sciences, 26, 29–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suter, S. (1982). Differences between deaf and hearing adults in task-related EEG asymmetries. Psychophysiology, 19, 124–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tattersall, G. D., and Johnston, R. D. (1984). Self organizing arrays for speech recognition. Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I., and Taylor, M. M. (1983). The psychology of reading. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. G. (1962). The behavioral basis of perception. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. M. (1973). The problem of stimulus structure in behavioral theory of perception. South African Journal of Psychology, 3, 23–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. M. (1974). Speculations on bilingualism and the cognitive network. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 2, 68–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. M. (1981/1984). The Bilateral Cooperative Model of reading: a human paradigm for artificial intelligence. In: Elithorn, A., and Banerji, R. (Eds.), Artificial and Human Intelligence. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Distributed as DCIEM Research Paper 81P - 4, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tweedy, J. R., and Lapinski, R. H. (1981). Facilitating word recognition: evidence for strategic and automatic factors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33 A, 51–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., and Wang, S. Y. (1977). Speech recoding in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 621–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wapner, W., Hamby, S., and Gardner, H. (1981). The role of the right hemisphere in the apprehension of complex linguistic materials. Brain and Language, 14, 15–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., and Seidenberg, M. S. (1986). Spelling-sound effects in reading: time-course and decision criteria. Memory and Cognition, 13, 557–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., and Bruck, M. (1984). Children’s and adults’ use of spellingsound information in three reading tasks. Memory and Cognition, 12, 293–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E., and Gardner, H. (1977). The comprehension of metaphor in brain-damaged patients. Brain, 100, 717–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yik, W.F. (1978). The effect of visual and acoustic similarity on short-term memory for Chinese words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 487–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1988 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Taylor, M.M. (1988). The Bilateral Cooperative Model of Reading. In: de Kerckhove, D., Lumsden, C.J. (eds) The Alphabet and the Brain. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-01093-8_18

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-01093-8_18

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-01095-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-01093-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics