Psychology of Literacy: East and West

  • Insup Taylor

Abstract

In this chapter, I discuss largely, though by no means exclusively, Far Eastern literacy. (The Far East will be henceforth referred to simply as “the East.”) And I tend to minimize the relations among literacy, brain, cognition, and culture. Literacy is simply obtaining sound and meaning from printed language; as such, literacy by itself may not have a great influence on cognition and culture. Literacy may have greater influence on cognition and culture, if it serves as “the magic key that unlocks the door to the wonderland of stories and information.” And, as I will argue, cortical activities are similar whether one reads in an Eastern or Western script.

Keywords

Corn Transportation Coherence Dition Xenon 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adams, M.J., and Huggins, A. W. E. (1985). The growth of children’s sight vocabulary: a quick test with educational and theoretical implications. Reading Research Quarterly 20262–281.Google Scholar
  2. Alegria, J., Pignot, E., and Morais, J. (1982). Phonetic analysis of speech and memory codes in beginning readers. Memory and Cognition, 10, 451–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amano, K. (1970). Formation of the act of analyzing phonemic structure of words and its relation to learning Japanese syllabic characters (Kanamoji). Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology 1876–89 (in Japanese with English abstract).Google Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. D., and Lewis, V.L. (1981). Inner active processes in reading: the inner voice, the inner ear, and the inner eye. In A. M. Lesgold, and C. A. Perfetti (Eds.) Interactive processes in reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Baddeley, A.D., Eldridge, M., and Lewis, V. (1981). The role of subvocalization in reading. Quarterly Journal of Psychology 33 a439–454.Google Scholar
  6. Baron, J., and Treiman, T. (1980). Use of orthography in reading and learning to read. In Kavanaugh, J. F., and Venezky, R. L. (Eds.), Orthography, reading and dyslexia. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  7. Becker, J. D. (1984). Computerized typing and editing can now be extended to all living languages of the world. Scientific American, 251, 96–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bliss, C. K. (1965). Semantography ( 2nd Ed. ). Sydney: Semantography.Google Scholar
  9. Bloodworth, D. (1967). The Chinese looking glass. London: Secker and Warburg.Google Scholar
  10. Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bradshaw, G. J., Hicks, R. E., and Ross, B. (1979). Lexical discrimination and letter-string identification in the two visual fields. Brain and Language, 8, 10–18.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brown, D.L. (1971). Some linguistic dimension in auditory blending. In Reading: the right to participate. (20th Yearbook of the National Reading Conference), Clemson, S. C.: National Reading Conference.Google Scholar
  13. Brownell, H.H., Michel, D., Powelson, J., and Gardner, H. (1983). Surprise but coherence: sensitivity to verbal humor in right-hemisphere patients. Brain and Language, 18, 20–27.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Calfee, R. C., Venezky, R. L., and Chapman, R. S. (1969). Pronunciation of synthetic words with predictable and unpredictable letter-sound correspondences. Technical Report 71, Wisconsin Research and Developmental Center for Cognitive Learning.Google Scholar
  15. Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press. Chong, J. Y., Han, S. J., and Kang, T. J. (1983). Hangul Word Processor III., JAE Consultants, 1983.Google Scholar
  16. Coleman, E. B. (1970). Collecting a data base for a reading technology. Journal of Educational Monograph, 61 (4), 1–23.Google Scholar
  17. Coleman, E.B., and Hahn, S.C. (1966). Failure to improve the readability with a vertical typography. Journal of Applied Psychology, 50, 434–436.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., and Marshall, J. C. (Eds.) (1980). Deep dyslexia: a review of the syndrome. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  19. Deloche, G., Andreewsky, E., and Desi, M. (1982). Surface dyslexia: a case report and some theoretical implications to reading models. Brain and Language, 15, 12–31.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Durkin, D. ( 1966 a). Children who read early. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
  21. Durkin, D. (1966 b). The achievement of pre-school readiness: two longitudinal studies. Reading Research Quarterly, 1, 5–36.Google Scholar
  22. Ehri, L.C. (1984). How orthography alters spoken language competencies in children learning to read and spell. In Downing, J., and Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  23. Endo, M., Shimizu, A., and Nakamura, I. (1981 a). Laterality differences in recognition of Japanese and Hangul words by monolinguals and bilinguals. Cortex, 17, 1–9.Google Scholar
  24. Endo, M., Shimizu A., and Nakamura, I. (1981 b). The influence of Hangul learning upon laterality difference in Hangul word recognition by native Japanese subjects. Brain and Language, 14, 114–119.Google Scholar
  25. Etiemble, M. (1973). L’ecriture. Paris: Gallimand.Google Scholar
  26. Feitelson, D.I. (1980). Relating instructional strategies to language idiosyncracies in Hebrew. In Kavanagh, J.F., and Venezky, R. L. (Eds.), Orthography, reading and dyslexia. Baltimore: University of Park Press.Google Scholar
  27. Flaugher, R. L. (1971). Patterns of test performance by high school students of four ethnic identities. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
  28. Fox, B., and Routh, D.K. (1976). Phonetic analysis and synthesis as word attack skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 70–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gainotti, G., Caltagirone, C., Micelli, G., and Masullo, C. (1981). Selective semantic-lexical impairment of language comprehension in right-brain damaged patients. Brain and Language, 13, 201–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gazzaniga, M. S. (1983). Right hemisphere language following brain bisection (a 20-year perspective). American Psychologist, 38, 525–549.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gelb, I. J. (1963). A study of writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1963.Google Scholar
  32. Gibson, E.J., Gibson, J.J., Pick, A.D., and Osser, H. A. (1962). A developmental study of the discrimination of letter-like forms. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 897–906.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Goody, J., and Watt, I. (1968). The consequences of literacy. In Literacy in traditional societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Gottesman, R. L., Croen, L., and Rotkin L. (1982). Urban second grade children: a profile of good and poor readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 15, 268–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hatta, T. (1978). Recognition of Japanese Kanji and Hirakana in the left and right visual fields. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 20, 51-59 (in Japanese with English abstract).Google Scholar
  36. Hatta, T. (1980). Kanji processing levels and cerebral hemisphere differences. Part IV 29,Osaka Educational University.Google Scholar
  37. Havelock, E. A. (1978). The Greek concept of justice: From its shadow in Homer to its substance in Plato. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Hayashi, R., and Hatta, T. (1982). Visual field differences in a deeper semantic processing task with Kanji stimuli. Japanese Psychological Research, 24, 111–117.Google Scholar
  39. Huang, J. T., and Liu, I. M. (1978) Paired-associate learning proficiency as a function of frequency count, meaningfulness, and imagery value in Chinese two-character ideograms Chinese Psychological Journal, 1978, 20, 5–17 (in Chinese with English abstract).Google Scholar
  40. Jensen, H. (1970). Sign, symbol and script. London: George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
  41. Just, M.A., and Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87, 329–354.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kates, B., MacNaughton, S., and Silverman, H. (1978). Handbook of Blissymbolics. Toronto: Blissymbol Communications Institute.Google Scholar
  43. Kratochvil, P. (1968). The Chinese language today. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
  44. Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S.A. (1982). The lateral distribution of event-related potentials during sentence processing. Neuropsychologia, 20, 579–590.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kyostio, O.K. (1980). Is learning to read easy in a language in which the grapheme-phoneme correspondences are regular? In Kavanagh, J. F., and Venezky R. L. (Eds.), Orthography, reading and dyslexia. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  46. Lassen, N.A., Ingvar, D.H., and Skinhoj, E. (1978). Brain function and blood flow. Scientific American, 239, 62–71.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lesser, G. S. (1976). Cultural differences in learning and thinking styles. In Messick, S. (Ed.), Individuality in learning and thinking. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  48. Lesser, G. S., Fifer, G., and Clark, D. H. (1965). Mental abilities of children from different social-class and cultural groups. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 30 (4).Google Scholar
  49. Liberman, I. Y., and Mann, V.A. (1981). Should reading instruction and remediation vary with the sex of the child? Status Report on Speech Research SR-65, Haskins Laboratories.Google Scholar
  50. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., and Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–212.Google Scholar
  51. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A. M., Fowler, C., and Fischer, F. W. (1977). Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning reader. In Reber, A. S., and Scarborough D. L. (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  52. Lin, Y. T. (1938). The wisdom of Confucius. New York: The Modern Library.Google Scholar
  53. Liu, I.M. (1978). Methods of acquiring Chinese vocabulary. Bulletin of the Sun Yat-sen Cultural Foundation, 22, 159–187.Google Scholar
  54. Liu, I. M. (1979). Personal communication, May 17.Google Scholar
  55. Liu, I. M. (1984). A survey of memorization requirement in Taipei primary and secondary schools. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  56. Liu, I. M., and Hsu, M. (1974). Measuring creative thinking in Taiwan by the Torrance Test. Testing and Guidence, 2, 108–109.Google Scholar
  57. Lynn, R. (1983). IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity. Nature, 297, 222–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of sounds. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Marin, O. S. M. (1980). CAT scans of five deep dyslexic patients. In Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., and Marshall, J. (Eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  60. Marshall, E. (1986). School reforms aim at creativity. Science, 233, 267–270.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Mishkin, M., and Forgays, D. G. (1952). Word recognition as a function of retinal locus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 43–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., and Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Muraishi, S., and Amano, K. (1972). Reading and writing abilities of preschoolers: a summary. Tokyo: The National Language Research Institute (in Japanese).Google Scholar
  64. Nakayama, S. (1973). The empirical tradition: science and technology in China. In Toynbee, A. (Ed.), Half the world: the history and culture of China and Japan. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  65. Navon, D., and Shimron, J. (1981). Does word naming involve grapheme-to-phoneme translation?: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 97–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Needham, J. (1954–1985). Science and civilization in China (Vol. I -5). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Needham, J. (1963). Poverties and triumphs of the Chinese scientific tradition. In Crombie, A. C. (Ed.), Scientific change. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  68. Ogden, J. A. (1984). Dyslexia in a right-handed patient with a posterior lesion of the right cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 22, 265–280.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Ornstein, R., Herron, J., Johnstone, J., and Swencionis, C. (1979). Differential right hemisphere involvement in two reading tasks. Psychophysiology, 16, 398–404.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Paivio, A. (1965). Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 32–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Paradis, M., Hagiwara, H., and Hildebrandt, N. (1985). Neurolinguistic aspects of the Japanese writing system. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  72. Park, R. (1978–1979). Performance on geometric figure copying tests as predictors of types of errors in decoding. Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 100–118.Google Scholar
  73. Paschal, B.J., Kuo, Y.-Y., and Schurr, K.T. (1980). Creative thinking in Indiana and Taiwan college students. Paper read at the 5th Conference of the International Association of Cross-cultural Psychology, 1980.Google Scholar
  74. Patel, P. G., and Patterson, P. (1983). Precocious reading acquisition: psycholinguistic development, IQ, and home background. First Language, 3, 139–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Premack, A.J., and Premack, D. (1972). Sarah, a young chimpanzee learns 130 words and a bit of grammar. Scientific American, 227, 92–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Premack, D. (1984). Upgrading a mind. In Bever, T.G., Carroll, J. M., and Miller, L.A. (Eds.), Talking minds: the study of language in cognitive science. Cambridge, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  77. Prohovnik, I., Hakansson, K., and Risberg, H. (1980). Observation on the functional significance of regional cerebral flow in `resting’ normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 18, 203–217.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Ross, E.D. (1983). Right-hemisphere lesions in disorders of affective language. In Kertesz, A. (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  79. Sakamoto, T., and Makita, K. (1973). Japan. In Downing, J. (Ed.), Comparative reading. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  80. Salapatek, P. (1968). Visual scanning of geometric figures by the human newborn. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 247–258.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Sasanuma, S. (1975). Kana and Kanji processing in Japanese aphasics. Brain and Language, 2, 369–383.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Sasanuma, S. (1980). Acquired dyslexia in Japanese: clinical features and underlying mechanisms. In Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., and Marshall, J. C. (Eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  83. Sasanuma, S., Itoh, M., Kobayashi, Y., and Mori, K. (1980). The nature of the task-stimulus interaction in the tachistoscopic recognition of Kana and Kanji words. Brain and Language, 9, 298–306.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Scribner, S., and Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  85. Steinberg, D.D., Harada, M., Tashiro, M., and Yamada, A. (1982). One congenitally deaf 1-year-old learn to read. Hearing Impaired, 376, 22–30 and 46 (in Japanese).Google Scholar
  86. Steinberg, D., Yoshida, K., and Yagi, R. (1985). Teaching reading to one and two year olds at home. The Science of Reading, 29,1-17 (in Japanese with English summary).Google Scholar
  87. Stern, J.A. (1978). Eye movements, reading, and cognition. In Senders, J. W., Fisher, D. F., and Monty, R. A. (Eds.), Eye movements and higher psychological functions (Vol. 2 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  88. Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., Lucker, G. W., Lee, S.-Y., Hsu, C.-C., and Kitamara, S. (1982). Reading disabilities: the case of Chinese, Japanese and English. Child Deve!opment, 53, 1164–1181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S.-Y., and Stigler, J. W. (1986). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children. Science, 231, 693–699.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Tanaka, T., and Yasufuku, J. (1975). Development of the cognition of letters (2). Osaka University of Education Report, 24, 85–99 (in Japanese with English abstract).Google Scholar
  91. Taylor, I. (1980). The Korean writing system: An alphabet? A syllabary? A logography? In Kolers, P.A., Wrolstad, M. E., and Bouma, H. (Eds.) Processing of visible language. v. 2. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  92. Taylor, I. (1986). The variety of scripts and reading. Proceedings of the 12th annual symposium of the Deseret Language and Linguistic Society. Provo, Utah; Brigham Young University.Google Scholar
  93. Taylor, I., and Taylor, M. M. (1983). The psychology of reading. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  94. Taylor, I., and Taylor, M.M. (1984). English function words. Unpublished paper.Google Scholar
  95. Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance tests of creative thinking: norms-technical manual. Princeton: Personnel Press.Google Scholar
  96. Tzeng, O. J. L., and Hung, D. (1980). Reading in a nonalphabetic writing system. In Kavanagh, J. F., and Venezky, R. L. (Eds.), Orthography, reading and dyslexia. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  97. Tzeng, O.J. L., Hung, D. L., and Wang, S-Y. (1977) Speech recoding in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 9, 621–630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Tzeng, O.J.L., Hung, D.L., Cotton, B., and Wang, S.-Y. (1979). Visual lateralization effects in reading Chinese characters. Nature, 282, 499–501.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Unger, J. (1977). Introduction: primary school reading texts and teaching methods in the wake of the cultural revolution. Chinese Education, 10, 4–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Valtin, R. (1984). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children learning to read and write. In Downing, J., and Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  101. Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Zaidel, E. ( 1978 a). Lexical organization in the right hemisphere. In Buser, P. A., and Rougeul-Buser, A. (Eds.), Cerebral correlates of conscious experience. (INSERM Symposium No. 6 ), Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical.Google Scholar
  103. Zaidel, E. ( 1978 b). Concepts of cerebral dominance in the split brain. In Buser, P. A., and Rougeul-Buser, A. (Eds.), Cerebral correlates of conscious experience (INSERM Symposium No. 6 ), Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical.Google Scholar
  104. Zaidel, E. (1983). A response to Gazzaniga: language in the right hemisphere. Convergent perspectives. American Psychologist, 38, 342–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Zaidel, E., and Peters, A. M. (1981). Phonological encoding and ideographic reading by the disconnected right hemisphere: Two case studies. Brain and Language, 14, 205–234.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Insup Taylor
    • 1
  1. 1.McLuhan Program in Culture and Technology (and Scarborough Campus)University of TorontoTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations