Skip to main content

Die Entwicklung kognitiver Fähigkeiten von Spielern am Beispiel der „Helix“

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
People Analytics im Profifußball

Part of the book series: Wirtschaft – Organisation – Personal ((WOP))

Zusammenfassung

In diesem Kapitel wird zunächst auf die Bedeutung der visuellen Wahrnehmung im Sport eingegangen, um danach zwei vielfach eingesetzte Methoden in diesem Bereich, das Multiple Object Tracking (MOT) und das 3D-MOT, vorzustellen. Auf deren Basis wurde ein realitätsnahes perzeptuell-kognitives Verfahren, das Helix-MOT, entwickelt. In Bezug auf dieses neue Diagnostik bzw. Trainingstool werden anschließend erste Studienergebnisse präsentiert sowie weitere Entwicklungspotenziale erörtert.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literatur

  • Abernethy, B. & Wood, J. M. (2001). Do generalized visual training programmes for sport really work? An experimental investigation. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19(3), 203-222. https://doi.org/10.1080/026404101750095376

  • Allen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D. & Milne, A. B. (2004). Attention and expertise in multiple target tracking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 337-347. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.975

  • Allen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D. G. & Milne, A. (2006). Multiple-target tracking: A role for working memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 1101-1116. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980543000097

  • Alvarez, G. A. & Cavanagh, P. (2005). Independent resources for attentional tracking in the left and right visual hemifields. Psychological Science, 16(8), 637-643. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01587.x

  • Alvarez, G. A. & Franconeri, S. L. (2007). How many objects can you track? Evidence for a resource-limited attentive tracking mechanism. Journal of Vision, 7(13), 14, 11-10. https://doi.org/10.1167/7.13.14

  • Alvarez, G. A., Gill, J. & Cavanagh, P. (2012). Anatomical constraints on attention: hemifield independence is a signature of multifocal spatial selection. Journal of Vision, 12(5), 9, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.5.9

  • Alves, M., Spaniol, F. & Erichsen, O. (2014). Visual skills of elite Brazilian soccer players. European Journal of Sports Science, 1-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ando, S., Kida, N. & Oda, S. (2001). Central and peripheral visual reaction time of soccer players and nonathletes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92(3), 786-794. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2001.92.3.786

  • Awh, E. & Pashler, H. (2000). Evidence for split attentional foci. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 834-846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, K., Allen, R. & McGeorge, P. (2010). Multiple-object tracking: Enhanced visuospatial representations as a result of experience. Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 208-214. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000026

  • Bettencourt, K. C. & Somers, D. C. (2009). Effects of target enhancement and distractor suppression on multiple object tracking capacity. Journal of Vision, 9(7), 9, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.7.9

  • Bridge, H., Thomas, O., Jbabdi, S. & Cowey, A. (2008). Changes in connectivity after visual cortical brain damage underlie altered visual function. Brain, 131(6), 1433-1444. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn063

  • Broadbent, D. P., Causer, J., Williams, A. M. & Ford, P. R. (2015). Perceptualcognitive skill training and its transfer to expert performance in the field: Future research directions. European Journal of Sport Science, 15(4), 322-331. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2014.957727

  • Cavanagh, P. & Alvarez, G. A. (2005). Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 349-354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.009

  • Chakravarthi, R. & Cavanagh, P. (2009). Bilateral field advantage in visual crowding. Vision Research, 49(13), 1638-1646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.026

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87-185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draganski, B. & May, A. (2008). Training-induced structural changes in the adult human brain. Behavioural Brain Research, 192(1), 137-142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.015

  • Du Toit, P., Kruger, P., Chamane, N., Campher, J. & Crafford, D. (2009). Sport vision assessment in soccer players: physical education and sport science. African Journal for Physical Health Education, Recreation and Dance, 15(4), 594-604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dye, M. W., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Differential development of visual attention skills in school-aged children. Vision Research, 50, 452–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faubert, J. (2013). Professional athletes have extraordinary skills for rapidly learning complex and neutral dynamic visual scenes. Scientific Reports, 3, 1154.https://doi.org/10.1038/srep01154

  • Faubert, J. & Sidebottom, L. (2012). Perceptual-cognitive training of athletes. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 6, 85-102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehd, H. M. & Seiffert, A. E. (2010). Looking at the center of the targets helps multiple object tracking. Journal of Vision, 10(4), 19 11-13. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.4.19

  • Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18, 850–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feria, C. S. (2012). The effects of distractors in multiple object tracking are modulated by the similarity of distractor and target features. Perception, 41(3), 287-304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fougnie, D. & Marois, R. (2006). Distinct capacity limits for attention and working memory: Evidence from attentive tracking and visual working memory paradigms. Psychological Science, 17(6), 526-534. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01739.x

  • Franconeri, S. L., Jonathan, S. V. & Scimeca, J. M. (2010). Tracking multiple objects is limited only by object spacing, not by speed, time, or capacity. Psychological Science, 21(7), 920-925. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373935

  • Franconeri, S. L., Lin, J. Y., Pylyshyn, Z. W., Fisher, B. & Enns, J. T. (2008). Evidence against a speed limit in multiple-object tracking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 802-808.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furley, P. A. & Memmert, D. (2012). Working memory capacity as controlled attention in tactical decision making. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 34(3), 322-344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006a). Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1465–1478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. S. & Bavelier, D. (2006b). Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: The case of action video game players. Cognition, 101, 217–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagemann, N., Lotz, S. & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2008). Wahrnehmungs-Handlungs- Kopplung beim taktischen Entscheidungstraining: Eine exploratorische Studie. Bewegung und Training, 2, 17-27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haywood, K. M. (1984). Use of the image-retina and eye-head movement visual systems during coincidence-anticipation performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 2(2), 139-144. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640418408729709

  • Holcombe, A. O. & Chen, W. Y. (2012). Exhausting attentional tracking resources with a single fast-moving object. Cognition, 123(2), 218-228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011./10.003

  • Horowitz, T. S., Birnkrant, R. S., Fencsik, D. E., Tran, L. & Wolfe, J. M. (2006). How do we track invisible objects? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 516-523. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193879

  • Horowitz, T. S., Klieger, S. B., Fencsik, D. E., Yang, K. K., Alvarez, G. A. & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Tracking unique objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(2), 172-184. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193740

  • Horowitz, T. S., Place, S. S., Van Wert, M. J. & Fencsik, D. E. (2007). The nature of capacity limits in multiple object tracking. Perception, 36, 9-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, C., Howe, P. D. & Little, D. R. (2012). Hemifield effects in multiple identity tracking. PloS One, 7(8), e43796. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043796

  • Intriligator, J. & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43(3), 171-216. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0755

  • Jendrusch, G., Kaczmarek, L., Lange, P., Lingelbach, B. & Platen, P. (2006). Zur visuellen Leistungsfähigkeit von Profi-Fußballspielern. In T. Henke, D. Schulz & P. Platen (Hrsg.), Sicherheit im Sport – „Ein Leben mit Sport – aber sicher“. Beiträge zum 4. Dreiländerkongress zur Sportunfallprävention, 21.-23. September 2006 in Bochum (S. 451-459). Köln: Strauß.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keane, B. P. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2006). Is motion extrapolation employed in multiple object tracking? Tracking as a low-level, non-predictive function. Cognitive Psychology, 52(4), 346-368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.12.001

  • Knudson, D. & Kluka, D. A. (1997). The impact of vision and vision training on sport performance. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 68(4), 17-24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemmink, K. A., Dijkstra, B. & Visscher, C. (2005). Effects of limited peripheral vision on shuttle sprint performance of soccer players. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100(1), 167-175. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.100.1.167-175

  • Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49(Suppl. 2), 467-477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahncke, H. W., Connor, B. B., Appelman, J., Ahsanuddin, O. N., Hardy, J. L., Wood, R. A.,. . . Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: A randomized, controlled study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(33), 12523-12528. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605194103

  • Makovski, T. & Jiang, Y. V. (2009a). Feature binding in attentive tracking of distinct objects. Visual Cognition, 17(1-2), 180-194. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280802211334

  • Makovski, T. & Jiang, Y. V. (2009b). The role of visual working memory in attentive tracking of unique objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(6), 1687-1697. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016453

  • Makovski, T., Vazquez, G. A. & Jiang, Y. V. (2008). Visual learning in multipleobject tracking. PloS One, 3(5), e2228. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002228

  • Mangine, G. T., Hoffman, J. R., Wells, A. J., Gonzalez, A. M., Rogowski, J. P., Townsend, J. R., Stout, J. R. (2014). Visual tracking speed is related to basketball- specific measures of performance in NBA players. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 28(9), 2406-2414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Memmert, D., Simons, D. J. & Grimme, T. (2009). The relationship between visual attention and expertise in sports. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10(1), 146-151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.06.002

  • Miles, H. C., Pop, S. R., Watt, S. J., Lawrence, G. P. & John, N. W. (2012). A review of virtual environments for training in ball sports. Computers & Graphics, 36(6), 714-726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, S. & Abernethy, B. (2012). Expert anticipatory skill in striking sports: A review and a model. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83(2), 175-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2012.10599848

  • Nan, W., Migotina, D., Wan, F., Lou, C. I., Rodrigues, J., Semedo, J., Da Rosa, A. C. (2014). Dynamic peripheral visual performance relates to alpha activity in soccer players. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 913, 911-918. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00913

  • Oksama, L. & Hyönä, J. (2004). Is multiple object tracking carried out automatically by an early vision mechanism independent of higher‐order cognition? An individual difference approach. Visual Cognition, 11(5), 631-671.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksama, L. & Hyönä, J. (2008). Dynamic binding of identity and location information: A serial model of multiple identity tracking. Cognitive Psychology, 56(4), 237-283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.03.001

  • Palmer, S. M. & Rosa, M. G. P. (2006). A distinct anatomical network of cortical areas for analysis of motion in far peripheral vision. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24(8), 2389-2405. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05113.x

  • Parsons, B., Magill, T., Boucher, A., Zhang, M., Zogbo, K., Berube, S., Faubert, J. (2014). Enhancing cognitive function using perceptual-cognitive training. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 47(1), 37-47. https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059414563746

  • Pinto, Y., Howe, P. D., Cohen, M. A. & Horowitz, T. S. (2010). The more often you see an object, the easier it becomes to track it. Journal of Vision, 10(10), 4, 1-15.https://doi.org/10.1167/10.10.4

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2004). Some puzzling findings in multiple object tracking: I. Tracking without keeping track of object identities. Visual Cognition, 11(7), 801-822.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3(3), 179-197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, K. M., Kelly, J. G. & Matthews, N. (2009). Bilateral attentional advantage on elementary visual tasks. Vision Research, 49(7), 691-701. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.01.005

  • Ripoll, H. (1991). The understanding-acting process in sport: The relationship between the semantic and the sensorimotor visual function. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22(3-4), 221-243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ripoll, H., Kerlirzin, Y., Stein, J. F. & Reine, B. (1995). Analysis of informationprocessing, decision-making, and visual strategies in complex problem-solving sport situations. Human Movement Science, 14(3), 325-349. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(95)00019-o

  • Rogers, T. J. & Landers, D. M. (2005). Mediating effects of peripheral vision in the life event stress/athletic injury relationship. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 27(3), 271-288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romeas, T., Guldner, A. & Faubert, J. (2016). 3D-Multiple object tracking training task improves passing decision-making accuracy in soccer players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 22, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.06.002

  • Schapschröer, M., Holzey, C., Bund, A. & Sickenberger, W. (2011). Trainierbarkeit der visuellen Wahrnehmung. Deutsche Optikerzeitung, 1, 92-96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholl, B. J. (2001). Objects and attention: The state of the art. Cognition, 80(1-2), 1-46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholl, B. J. (2009). What have we learned about attention from multiple object tracking (and vice versa)? In D. Dedrick & L. Trick (Hrsg.), Computation, cognition, and Pylyshyn (S. 49-78). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholl, B. J. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1999). Tracking multiple items through occlusion: Clues to visual objecthood. Cognitive Psychology, 38(2), 259-290. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0698

  • Scholl, B. J., Pylyshyn, Z. W. & Feldman, J. (2001). What is a visual object? Evidence from target merging in multiple object tracking. Cognition, 80(1-2), 159-177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwab, S. & Memmert, D. (2012). The impact of a sports vision training program in youth field hockey players. Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, 11(4), 624.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shim, W. M., Alvarez, G. A. & Jiang, Y. V. (2008). Spatial separation between targets constrains maintenance of attention on multiple objects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 390-397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smeeton, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J. & Ward, P. (2005). The relative effectiveness of various instructional approaches in developing anticipation skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11(2), 98-110. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898x.11.2.98

  • Söhnlein, K. & Borgmann S. (2018). Diagnostik von Exekutivfunktionen im Fußball.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, I., Yu, J. J., Fend, J., & Marshman, J. (2009). Women match men when learning a spatial skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1097 – 1103

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, L. E. & Seiffert, A. E. (2010). Self-motion impairs multiple-object tracking. Cognition, 117(1), 80-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.07.002

  • Tombu, M. & Seiffert, A. E. (2008). Attentional costs in multiple-object tracking. Cognition, 108(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.014

  • Trick, L. M., Jaspers-Fayer, F., & Sethi, N. (2005). Multiple-object tracking in children. The “Catch the Spies” task. Cognitive Development, 20, 373–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., Mazyn, L. & Philippaerts, R. M. (2007). The effects of task constraints on visual search behavior and decision-making skill in youth soccer players. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29(2), 147-169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viswanathan, L. & Mingolla, E. (2002). Dynamics of attention in depth: Evidence from multi-element tracking. Perception, 31(12), 1415-1437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherill, G. B. (1963). Sequential estimation of quantal response curves. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 25(1), 1-48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M. (2000). Perceptual skill in soccer: Implications for talent identification and development. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18(9), 737-750.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M. & Davids, K. (1997). Assessing cue usage in performance contexts: A comparison between eye-movement and concurrent verbal report methods. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29(3), 364-375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J., North, J. S. & Barton, G. (2006). Perceiving patterns of play in dynamic sport tasks: Investigating the essential information underlying skilled performance. Perception, 35(3), 317-332. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5310

  • Yantis, S. (1992). Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization. Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 295-340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelinsky, G. J. & Todor, A. (2010). The role of “rescue saccades” in tracking objects through occlusions. Journal of Vision, 10(14), 29, 01-13. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.14.29

  • Zhang, X., Yan, M. & Yangang, L. (2009). Differential performance of Chinese volleyball athletes and nonathletes on a multiple-object tracking task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109(3), 747-756. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.109.3.747-756

  • Zwierko, T. (2007). Differences in peripheral perception between athletes and nonathletes. Journal of Human Kinetics, 19, 53-62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwierko, T., Głowacki, T. & Osiński, W. (2008). The effect of specific anaerobic exercises on peripheral perception in handball players. Kinesiologia Slovenica, 14(1), 68–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwierko, T., Osiński, W., Lubiński, W., Czepita, D. & Florkiewicz, B. (2010). Speed of visual sensorimotor processes and conductivity of visual pathway in volleyball players. Journal of Human Kinetics, 23, 21-27.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tobias Kittelberger .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Kittelberger, T. (2018). Die Entwicklung kognitiver Fähigkeiten von Spielern am Beispiel der „Helix“. In: Lanwehr, R., Mayer, J. (eds) People Analytics im Profifußball. Wirtschaft – Organisation – Personal. Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-21256-8_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-21256-8_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-658-21255-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-658-21256-8

  • eBook Packages: Business and Economics (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics