Skip to main content

Zugriff auf Wortbedeutungen

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Die Natur der Sprache
  • 3693 Accesses

Zusammenfassung

Wie auf lexikalische Bedeutungen Zugriff genommen wird, hängt von verschiedenen Variablen ab, einschließlich der Eingangsmodalität, morphosyntaktischer Strukturen, perzeptueller Merkmale und Abstraktheitsgrad. Sensorische und kategorienspezifische Störungen können durch Kaskadenprozesse innerhalb eines modalitätsunabhängigen lexikalisch-konzeptuellen Systems vorhergesagt werden. Lexikalische Bedeutungen sind dezentralisiert repräsentiert. Auch wenn die Bedeutung primär perzeptueller Natur ist, konzeptuelle Mediationsprozesse sind offensichtlich stets beim semantischen Zugriff beteiligt. Welche corticalen Areale aktiviert werden, hängt von Berechnungskosten und der Art des mit der lexikalischen Einheit assoziierten Inhalts ab. Die fMRT-Daten verdeutlichen, dass das Broca-Areal dann aktiviert wird, wenn der Zugriff auf idiomatische Phrasen höhere Berechnungskosten erfordert. Weiterhin stellt die „Geschwind-Region“ (Gyrus supramarginalis und Gyrus angularis) die Schaltstelle zwischen präfrontalen und temporookzipitalen Regionen dar und trägt entscheidend zu der Integration multi-sensorischer Erfahrung bei. Diese Region, die aus evolutionärer Sicht relativ jung zu sein scheint, ist vermutlich insbesondere an Prozessen beteiligt, die Aufmerksamkeit und Bewusstsein erfordern.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literatur

  • Aggujaro, S., Crepaldi, D., Pistarini, C., Taricco, M., & Luzzatti, C. (2006). Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired retrieval of verbs and nouns: Interaction of grammatical class, imageability and actionality. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19(3), 175–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahrens, K, Liu, H. L., Lee, C. Y., Gong, S. P., Fang, S. Y., & Hsu, Y. Y. (2007). Functional MRI of conventional and anomalous metaphors in Mandarin Chinese. Brain and Language, 100, 163–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D. A. (1985). Distributed memory, modular systems and dysphasia. In S. K. Newman & R. Epstein (eds.), Current perspective in dysphasia. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C.D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, M. F. (1982). Optic aphasia: a process of interaction between vision and language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 35–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, M. F., Saillant, B., Meininger, V., & Lhermitte, F. (1978). Bilateral tactile aphasia: a tacto-verbal dysfunction. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 101(3), 381–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Cox, R. W., Rao, S. M., & Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(1), 353–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobrow, S. A., & Bell, S. M. (1973). On catching on to idiomatic expressions. Memory & Cognition, 1(3), 343–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohrn, I. C., Altmann, U., & Jacobs, A. M. (2012). Looking at the brains behind figurative language – a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on metaphor, idiom, and irony processing. Neuropsychologia, 50(11), 2669–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bookheimer, S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annual Review Neuroscience, 25, 151–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottini, G., Corcoran, R., Sterzi, R., Paulesu, E., Schenone, P., Scarpa, P., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1994). The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language. A positron emission tomography activation study. Brain, 117(6), 1241–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde. Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Bart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, H. H., Potter, H. H., Michelow, D., & Gardner, H. (1984). Sensitivity to lexical denotation and connotation in brain-damaged patients: a double dissociation? Brain and Language, 29, 310–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, H. H., Simpson, T. L., Bihrle, A. M., Potter, H. H., & Gardner, H. (1990). Appreciation of metaphoric alternative word meanings by left and right brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 28(4), 375–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, E. (2004). Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the genus Homo. Journal of Human Evolution, 47(5), 279–303. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.03.009.

  • Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M.-C., & Decety, J. (2000). A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 11, 157–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, E. (2010). Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus: A neurofunctional perspective. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S77–S88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., & Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(3), 355–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Vermonti, T., & Schwartz, M. F. (2000). Function andmanipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: knowing “what for”but not “how”. Neurocase, 6, 83–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacciari, C., & Glucksberg, S. (1991). Understanding idiomatic expressions: The contribution of word meanings. In: G. B. Simpson (ed.), Understanding word and sentence. North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacciari, C., & Tabossi, P. (1988). The comprehension of idioms. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 668–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calkins, M. W. (1893). A statistical study of pseudo-chromesthesia and of mental-forms. The American Journal of Psychology, 5(4), 439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, D. (2001). Functional neuroimaging studies of syntactic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 297–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1998). Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 541–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B. C., & Romani, C. (1990). The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7(3), 161–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage, 33(2), 430–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catani, M., Jones, D. K., & Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57(1), 8–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copland, D. A., Chenery, H. J., & Murdoch, B. F. (2002). Hemispheric contributions to lexical ambiguity resolution: Evidence from individuals with complex language impairment following left-hemisphere lesions. Brain and Language, 81, 131–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crepaldi, D., Berlingeri, M., & Luzzatti, C. (2013). Clustering the lexicon in the brain: a meta-analysis of the neurofunctional evidence on noun and verb processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, B., Lima, S., & Schweigert, W. (1993). Idioms in sentences: Effects of frequency, literalness, and familiarity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22(1), 59–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniele, A., Giustolisi, L., Silveri, M. C., Colosimo, C., & Gainotti, G. (1994). Evidence for a possible neuroanatomical basis for lexical processing of nouns and verbs. Neuropsychologia, 32(11), 1325–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (1999). Form and content. Neuron, 24, 427–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Renzi, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (1995). Sparing of verbs and preserved, but ineffectual reading in a patient with impaired word production. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 31(4), 619–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demb, J., Desmond, J., Wagner, A., Vaidya, C., Glover, G., & Gabrieli, J. (1995). Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: A functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 5870–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Jr., Redfern, B. B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92(1–2), 145–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., & Just, M. A. (2006). Brain correlates of discourse processing: An fMRI investigation of irony and conventional metaphor comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2348–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., Hammond, K. M., Mehta, Z., & Ratcliff, G. (1989). Category-specificity and modality-specificity in semantic memory. Neuropsychologia, 27(2), 193–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foldi, N. S. (1987). Appreciation of pragmatic interpretations of indirect commands. Brain and Language, 31, 88–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frattali, C., Hanna, R., McGinty, A. S., Gerber, L., Wesley, R., Grafman, J., & Coelho, C. (2007). Effect of prefrontal cortex damage on resolving lexical ambiguity in text. Brain and Language, 102, 99–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2003). The Roots of Empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 36(4), 171–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2007). Before and below “theory of mind”: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 659–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H., & Brownell, H. H. (1986.). Right hemisphere communication battery. Boston, MA: Psychological Service VAMC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (1980). Spilling the beans on understanding and memory for idioms in conversation. Memory & Cognition, 8(2), 149–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S. J., Williamson, I. D. M., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and non-social functions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 217–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S. J., Spengler, S., Simons, J. S., Steele, J. D., Lawrie, S. M., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta- analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 932–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giora, R. (1997). Understanding figurative language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(1), 183–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Sonty, S., Parrish, T. B., & Mesulam, M.-M. (2005). Language network specializations: An analysis with parallel task designs and functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage, 26(4), 975–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G. (2003). Apraxia and beyond: life and work of Hugo Liepmann. Cortex 39(3), 509–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H. (1994). Category-specific lexical dissociations. Linguistische Berichte. Special Issue: Linguistics and Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 49–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In: P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press, pp. 41–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral and Brain Science, 23, 1–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y., & Santi, A. (2008). The battle for Broca’s region. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 474–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(7), 4259–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J., & Gordon, B. (1992). Neural subsystems for object knowledge. Nature, 359(6390), 60–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J., Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A. (1985). Category-specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction. Nature, 316(6027), 439–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D. (1990). Sprachprozesse und Wissensstrukturen [German]. Opladen: Westdeutscher Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D. (1992). Lexical semantics and aphasia: A state-of-the-art review. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 7(1), 1–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D., Burrington, D. F. H., & Gupta, G. A. (1994). Semantic activation for lexical perception. Linguistische Berichte (4): Linguistics and Cognitive Neuroscience, 245–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D., & Buračas, G. (2009). The neural substrates of spoken idiom comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(9), 1370–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D., & Swinney, D. (2000). The processing of fixed expressions during sentence comprehension. In A. Cienki, B. J. Luka & M. B. Smith (eds.), Conceptual structure, discourse, and language. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D. G. (2004). Spared idiom comprehension in aphasia: A single-case approach. Brain and Language 89 (1), 207–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D. G. (2008). On Idioms: Cornerstones of a neurological model of language processing. Journal of Cognitive Science 9(2), 193–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillert, D. G. (2011). Nimm’s nicht so wörtlich [German]. Gehirn und Geist, Nov., 70–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, E. M., Arman, A. C., Ramachandran, V. S., & Boynton, G. M. (2005). Individual differences among grapheme-color synesthetes: Brain-behavior correlations. Neuron, 45(6), 975–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., Hodsoll, J., & Riddoch, M. J. (2009). Fractionating the binding process: neuropsychological evidence from reversed search efficiencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 627–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J., & Price, C. J. (1997). Top-down processes in object identification: evidence from experimental psychology, neuropsychology and functional anatomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 352(1358), 1275–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J., & Quinlan, P. T. (1988). Cascade processes in picture identification. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5(1), 67–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husain, M., & Nachev, P. (2007). Space and the parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 30–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., & Hannequin, D. (1990). Right hemisphere and verbal communication. Springer Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127(8), 1811–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2002). The brain circuitry of syntactic comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 350–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempler, D., Van Lancker, D., & Read, S. (1988). Proverb and idiom comprehension in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 2(1), 38–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kircher, T. T., Brammer, M., Tous Andreu, N., Williams, S. C., & McGuire, P. K. (2001). Engagement of right temporal cortex during processing of linguistic context. Neuropsychologia, 39(8), 798–809.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691–702.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauro, L. J. R., Tettamanti, M., Cappa, S. F., & Papagno, C. (2008). Idiom comprehension: A prefrontal task? Cerebral Cortex, 18, 162–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L., Friston, K., & Horwitz, B. (2006). Large-scale neural models and dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage, 30(4), 1243–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. S., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Metaphorical vs. literal word meanings: fMRI evidence against a selective role of the right hemisphere. Neuroimage, 29(2), 536–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libon, D. J., Rascovsky, K., Powers, J., Irwin, D. J., Boller, A., Weinberg, D., McMillan, C. T., & Grossman, M. (2013). Comparative semantic profiles in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 136(8), 2497–509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liepmann, H. (1905). Ueber Störungen des Handelns bei Gehirnkranken. S. Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., Banfield, J. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2004). Medial Prefrontal Activity Predicts Memory for Self. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 647–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 102(1–3), 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marques, J. F., Raposo, A., & Almeida, J. (2013). Structural processing and category-specific deficits. Cortex, 49(1), 266–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Category-specificity and the brain: the sensory-motor model of semantic representations of objects. In M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1023–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of category- specific knowledge. Nature, 379(6566), 649–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mashal, N., Faust, M., & Hendler, T. (2005). The role of the right hemisphere in processing nonsalient metaphorical meanings: application of principal components analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia, 43(14), 2084–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2008). Hemispheric differences in processing the literal interpretation of idioms: converging evidence from behavioral and fMRI studies. Cortex, 44(7), 848–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2007). Lexical ambiguity in sentence comprehension. Brain Research, 1146(18), 115–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, R. A., Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (2003). Ambiguity in the brain: What brain imaging reveals about the processing of syntactically implicit sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1319–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., Pathman, T., & Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science, 9(3), 316–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, R., & Warrington, E. K. (1985). Category specificity in an agrammatic patient: the relative impairment of verb retrieval and comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 23(6), 709–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1988). Evidence for modality-specific meaning systems in the brain. Nature, 334(6181), 428–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1994). Disorders of semantic memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 346(1315), 89–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, M., Glucksberg, S., & Cacciari, C. (1994). Semantic productivity and idiom comprehension. Discourse Processes, 17, 167–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K. (1999). The prefrontal cortex: complex neural properties for complex behavior. Neuron, 22, 15–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., Freedman, D. J., & Wallis, J. D. (2002). The prefrontal cortex: categories, concepts and cognition. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 29, 357(1424), 1123–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichelli, P., Grafman, J., Pietrini, P., Clark, K., Lee, K. Y., & Miletich, R. (1995). Where the brain appreciates the moral of a story. Neuroreport, 6(17), 2309–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunn, J. A., Gregory, L. J., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C. R., Parslow, D. M., Morgan, M. J., Morris, R. G., Bullmore, E. T., Baron-Cohen, S., & Gray, J. A. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: activation of V4/V8 by spoken words. Nature Neuroscience, 5(4), 371–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Pineda, J. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The human mirror neuron system: A link between action observation and social skills. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 62–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Finocchiaro, C., Shapiro, K., Gangitano, M., Caramazza, A., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2004). All talk and no action: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex activation during action word production. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 374–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Romero, L., & Papagno, C. (2004). Left but not right temporal involvement in opaque idiom comprehension: a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 848–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orban, G. A., Claeys, K., Nelissen, K., Smans, R., Sunaert, S., Todd, J. T., Wardak, C., Durand, J. B., & Vanduffel, W. (2006). Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates. Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2647–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Schallert, D. L., Reynolds, R. E., & Antos, S. J. (1978). Interpreting metaphors and idioms: some effects of context on comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 465–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., & Caporali, A. (2007). Testing idiom comprehension in aphasic patients: The effects of task and idiom type. Brain and Language, 100(2), 208–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., Curti R., Rizzo, S., Crippa, F., & Colombo, M. R. (2006). Is the right hemisphere involved in idiom comprehension? A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 20(5), 598–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., Oliveri, M., & Romero, L. (2002). Neural correlates of idiom comprehension. Cortex, 38, 895–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, L., Van Lancker-Sidtis, D. R., Schieffer, B., Dietrich, R., & Brown, W. (2003). Communicative deficits in agenesis of the corpus callosum: Nonliteral language and affective prosody. Brain and Language, 85, 313–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Bettinardi, V., Bressi, S., Gorno-Tempini, M., Matarrese, M., & Fazio, F. (1995). Different neural systems for the recognition of animals and man-made tools. Neuroreport, 6(12), 1637–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. R., & Burgess, C. (1993). Syntactic and semantic processing during idiom comprehension: neurolinguistic and psycholinguistic dissociation. In C. Cacciari & P. Tabossi (eds.), Idioms: processing, structure and interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. R., Burgess, C., Dell, G. S., & Eberhard, K. M. (2001). Dissociation between syntactic and semantic processing during idiom comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27(5), 1223–1237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pineda, J. A. (ed.) (2009). Mirror neuron systems – The role of mirroring processes in social cognition. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. A., Wagner, A. D., Prull, M. W., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1999). Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 10(1), 15–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Functional links between motor and language systems. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(3), 793–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia – a window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, A. M., Leube, D. T., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Kircher, T. T. (2004). Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 395–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, A. M., Mutschler, D. E., & Erb, M. (2012). Where in the brain is nonliteral language? A coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. NeuroImage, 63(1), 600–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Coltheart, M., & Funnell, E. (1988). Semantic systems or system? Neuropsychological evidence re-examined. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roskies, A. L., Fiez, J. A., Balota, D. A., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (2001). Task-dependent modulation of regions in the left inferior frontal cortex during semantic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(6), 829–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahin, N. T., Pinker, S., Cash, S. S., Schomer, D., & Halgren, E. (2009). Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area. Science, 326(5951), 445–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1987). Impairments of semantic processing: multiple dissociations. In M. Coltheart, G. Sartori & R. Job (eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shibata, M., Toyomura, A., Itoh, H., & Abe, J. (2010). Neural substrates of irony comprehension: A functional MRI study. Brain Research, 1308, 114–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sotillo, M., Carretie, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Tapia, M., Mercado, F., Lopez-Martin, S., & Albert, J. (2005). Neural activity associated with metaphor comprehension: spatial analysis. Neuroscience Letters, 373(1), 5–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2001). Précis of relevance: communication and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10(4), 697–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stowe, L. A., Broere, C. A. J., Paans, A. M. J., Wijers, A. A., Mulder, G., Vaalburg, W., & Zwarts, F. (1998). Localizing components of a complex task: sentence processing and working memory. Neuroreport, 9(13), 2995–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stringaris, A. K., Medford, N. C., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M. J., & David, A. S. (2007). Deriving meaning: Distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain and Language, 100, 150–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Subramaniam, K., Beeman, M., & Mashal, N. (2013). Positively valenced stimuli facilitate creative novel metaphoric processes by enhancing medial prefrontal cortical activation. Frontiers in Cognitive Science, 4, 211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D., & Cutler, A. (1979). The access and processing of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 523–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P., & Zardon, F. (1993). The activation of idiomatic meaning in spoken language comprehension. In C. Cacciari & P. Tabossi (eds.), Idioms: Processing, structure and interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P., & Zardon, F. (1995). The activation of idiomatic meaning. In M. Everaert, E.-J. van der Linden, A. Schenk & R. Schreuder (eds.), Idioms: structural and psychological perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titone, D. A., & Connine, C. M. (1994). Comprehension of idiomatic expressions: effects of predictability and literality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1126–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tompkins, C. A., Boada, R., & McGarry, K. (1992). The access and processing of familiar idioms by brain-damaged and normally aging adults. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 35(3), 626–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turken, A. U., & Dronkers, N. F. (2011). The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5(1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Uchiyama, H., Seki, A., Kageyama, H., Saito, D. N., Koeda, T., Ohno, K., & Sadato, N. (2006). Neural substrates of sarcasm: A functional magnetic-resonance imaging study. Brain Research, 1124(1): 100–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lancker, D. R., & Kempler, D. (1987). Comprehension of familiar phrases by left- but not by right-hemisphere damaged patients. Brain and Language, 32, 265–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lancker-Sidtis, D. (2004). When novel sentences spoken or heard for the first time in the history of the universe are not enough: toward a dual-process model of language. International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders, 39(1): 1–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A., Desmond, J., Demb, J., Glover, G., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1997). Semantic repetition priming for verbal and pictorial knowledge. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 714–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wakusawa, K., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., Yokoyama, H., Tsuchiya, S., Inuma, K., & Kawashima, R. (2007). Comprehension of implicit meanings in social situations involving irony: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage, 37(4), 1417–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107(3), 829–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E., & Gardner, H. (1977). The comprehension of metaphor in brain-damaged patients. Brain, 100, 719–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E. (1988). The point of words: Children’s understanding of metaphor and irony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., Marrett, S., & Neelin, P. (1992). A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 12, 900–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zempleni, M. Z., Haverkort, M., Renken, R., & Stowe, L. A. (2007). Evidence for bilateral involvement in idiom comprehension: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 34(3), 1280–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zempleni, M. Z., Renken, R., Hoeks, C. J., Hoogduin, J. M., & Stowe, L. A. (2007). Semantic ambiguity processing in sentence context: Evidence from event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 34, 1270–79.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dieter Hillert .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hillert, D. (2018). Zugriff auf Wortbedeutungen. In: Die Natur der Sprache. Springer, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-20113-5_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-20113-5_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Wiesbaden

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-658-20112-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-658-20113-5

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics