Skip to main content

Implicit and Explicit Measures: What Their Dissociations Reveal About the Workings of Advertising

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Advances in Advertising Research (Vol. VI)

Part of the book series: European Advertising Academy ((EAA))

Abstract

Consider a scenario in which you participate in a study on advertising effects. You are instructed to attend to an advertisement, after which you are asked about your memory for the advertisement and advertised brand, your attitude towards the ad and brand, and your intention to purchase the advertised brand. Sometimes, you will also be asked to reflect about the proposed process that is considered to underlie advertising effects, with questions along the lines of ‘do you think that this advertisement has affected your attitude about the brand?’ or ‘do you think the music in the advertisement affected how positive you feel about the brand?’.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bowers, J. S., and D.L. Schacter (1990), “Implicit memory and test awareness,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 404–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunel, F. F.; Tietje, B. C. and A.G. Greenwald (2004), “Is the implicit association test a valid and valuable measure of implicit consumer social cognition?” in: Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 385–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carraro, L.; Gawronski, B. and L. Castelli (2010), “Losing on all fronts: The effects of negative versus positive person-based campaigns on implicit and explicit evaluations of political candidates,” in: British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(3), 453–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L. (2005), “The role of conscious awareness in consumer behavior,” in: Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 203–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., and G.J. Fitzsimons (2011), “Nonconscious consumer psychology,” in: Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 1–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A.; Smith, P. K.; Van Baaren, R. B. and D.H. Wigboldus (2005), “The unconscious consumer: Effects of environment on consumer behavior,” in: Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 193–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F.; Kawakami, K. and S.L. Gaertner (2002), “Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction,” in: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 62–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duke, C. R., and L. Carlson (1993), “A conceptual approach to alternative memory measures for advertising effectiveness,” in: Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 15(2), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2001), “On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview,” in: Cognition & Emotion, 15(2), 115–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H.; Jackson, J. R.; Dunton, B. C. and C.J. Williams (1995), “Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline?” in: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013–1027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. M.; Chartrand, T. L. and G.J. Fitzsimons (2008), “Automatic effects of brand exposure on motivated behavior: How apple makes you “think different”,” in: Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 21–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forehand, M. R. and A. Perkins (2005), “Implicit assimilation and explicit contrast: A Set/Reset model of response to celebrity voice‐overs,” in: Journal of Consumer Research, 32(3), 435–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galdi, S.; Arcuri, L. and B. Gawronski (2008), “Automatic mental associations predict future choices of undecided decision-makers,” in: Science, 321(5892), 1100–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B. (2009), “Ten frequently asked questions about implicit measures and their frequently supposed, but not entirely correct answers,” in: Canadian Psychology, 50(3), 141–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B. and G.V. Bodenhausen (2006), “Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change,” in: Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B. and E.P. LeBel (2008), “Understanding patterns of attitude change: When implicit measures show change, but explicit measures do not,” in: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1355–1361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B.; LeBel, E. P. and K.R. Peters (2007), “What do implicit measures tell us? Scrutinizing the validity of three common assumptions,” in: Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(2), 181–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, B. (2008), “Can evaluative conditioning change attitudes toward mature brands? New evidence from the implicit association test,” in: Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 178–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P. and G. Mandler (1984), “Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable,” in: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(5), 553–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P. and D.L. Schacter (1985), “Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(3), 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P.; Squire, L. R. and G. Mandler (1984), “The information that amnesic patients do not forget,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(1), 164–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G.; McGhee, D. E. and J.L.K. Schwartz (1998), “Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test,” in: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Häfner, M. and D. Trampe (2009), “When thinking is beneficial and when it is not: The effects of thin and round advertising models,” in: Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(4), 619–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, R. (2001), “Low involvement processing-a new model of brand communication,” in: Journal of Marketing Communications, 7(1), 27–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, D.; De Houwer, J. and P. Eelen (1994), “The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory,” in: Cognition & Emotion, 8(6), 515–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991), “A process dissociation framework – Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory,” in: Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 513–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. and M. Dallas (1981), “On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110(3), 306–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maison, D.; Greenwald, A. G. and R.H. Bruin (2004), “Predictive validity of the implicit association test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes, and behavior,” in: Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 405–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merikle, P. M. and E.M. Reingold (1991), “Comparing direct (explicit) and indirect (implicit) measures to study unconscious memory,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 224–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E. and T.D. Wilson (1977), “Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes,” in: Psychological Review, 84(3), 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redker, C.; Gibson, B. and I. Zimmerman (2013), “Liking of movie genre alters the effectiveness of background product placements,” in: Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(3), 249–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987), “Implicit memory – History and current status,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 13(3), 501–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (1999), “Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers,” in: American Psychologist, 54(2), 93–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, S. and H.S. Krishnan (2001), “Memory-based measures for assessing advertising effects: A comparison of explicit and implicit memory effects,” in: Journal of Advertising, 30(3), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F. and R. Deutsch (2004), “Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior,” in: Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E.; Schacter, D. L. and H.A. Stark (1982), “Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory,” in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(4), 336–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Reijmersdal, E. A.; Rozendaal, E. and M. Buijzen (2012), “Brand prominence in advergames: Effects on Children’s explicit and implicit memory,” in: Eisend, M.; Langner, T.; Okazaki, S. (2012) (eds.): Advances in advertising research, Gabler Verlag, Vol. III, 321–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandeberg, L.; Murre, J. M. J.; Voorveld, H. A. M. and E.G. Smit (forthcoming), “Effects of cross-media advertising: Explicit versus implicit memory measures,” in: International Journal of Advertising.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K. and L. Weiskrantz (1968), “New method of testing long-term retention with special reference to amnesic patients,” in: Nature, 217, 972–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K. and L. Weiskrantz (1970), “Amnesic syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval?” in: Nature, 228, 628–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wennekers, A. M.; Vandeberg, L.; Zoon, K. and E.A. van Reijmersdal (2015), “Distinguishing between explicit and implicit brand attitudes in brand placement research,” in Verlegh, P. W. J.; Voorveld, H. A. M.; Eisend, M. (2015) (eds.): EAA advances in advertising research, Wiesbaden Gabler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D. and N. Brekke (1994), “Mental contamination and mental correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations,” in: Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 117–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, M. and D.R. Roskos-Ewoldsen (2007), “The effectiveness of brand placements in the movies: Levels of placements, explicit and implicit memory, and brand‐choice behavior,” in: Journal of Communication, 57(3), 469–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, M.; Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R.; Dinu, L. and L.M. Arpan (2006), “The effectiveness of "in-game" advertising: Comparing college students' explicit and implicit memory for brand names,” in: Journal of Advertising, 35(4), 143–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoo, C. Y. (2007), “Implicit memory measures for web advertising effectiveness,” in: Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 84(1), 7–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoo, C. Y. (2008), “Unconscious processing of web advertising: Effects on implicit memory, attitude toward the brand, and consideration set,” in: Journal of Interactive Marketing, 22(2), 2–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980), “Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences,” in: American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa Vandeberg .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Vandeberg, L., Wennekers, A., Murre, J., Smit, E. (2016). Implicit and Explicit Measures: What Their Dissociations Reveal About the Workings of Advertising. In: Verlegh, P., Voorveld, H., Eisend, M. (eds) Advances in Advertising Research (Vol. VI). European Advertising Academy. Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-10558-7_21

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics