Skip to main content

Abstract

The process of language comprehension consists of a number of distinct subprocesses which operate on different knowledge sources. The process from the input to the neutral representation of the meaning that has been conveyed may be described as follows: as a first step peripheral input systems will have to process the auditory or visual language input, the phonological processing level will analyze the input and build up a phonological representation which allows access to the lexicon and identification of given lexical entries. At the lexical level morphological aspects and semantic aspects become evident. When processing lexical elements in sentential context syntactic aspects such as the word’s syntactic category comes into play. Moreover, during sentence processing the system not only has access to those words that refer to the outside world, i.e. the content words, but access to function words also. These latter words indicate the grammatical relations holding between the content words and are therefore of major importance for a sentence’s interpretation. Grammatical relations may, depending on the language, also be marked by inflectional morphology. Function words and inflectional elements constitute the class of minor category items, the so-called closed class, whereas content words constitute the class of major category items, the so-called open class. The former class plus the information about a word’s syntactic category allow the building of a syntactic structure at the sentence level.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Auerbach, S.H., Allard, T., Naeser, M., Alexander, MLP. & Albert, M.L. (1982). Pure word deafness: Analysis of a case with bilateral lesions and a defect at the pre-phonemic level. Brain, 105, 271–300.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, S.E. & Rugg, M.D. (1990). Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures. Brain and Cognition, 14, 201–212.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Basso, A., Casati, G. & Vignolo, L.A. (1977). Phonemic identification defect in aphasia. Cortex, 13, 85–95.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, M.E & Derousné, J. (1979). Phonological alexia: Three dissociations. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 42, 1115–1124.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, R. & Caramazza, A. (1980). A redefinition of the syndrom of Broca’s aphasia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 225–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, R.S. & Mitchum, C.C. (1990). Auditory and lexical information sources in immediate recall: Evidence from a patient with a deficit to the phonological shortterm store. In: G. Vallar & T. Shallice (eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierwisch, M. & Schreuder, R. (1992). From concepts to lexical items. Cognition, 42, 23–60.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J.R., Rao, S.M. & Tammeke, T.A. (1994). Functional MRI of human auditory cortex. Annales of Neurology, 35, 662–672.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein, S.E., Baker, E. & Goodglass, H. (1977a). Phonological factors in auditory comprehension in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 15, 19–30.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein, S., Cooper, W.E., Zurif, E.B. & Caramazza, A. (1977b). The perception and production of voice-onset time in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 15, 371–383.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein, S.E., Milberg, W. & Schrier, R. (1982). Semantic processing in aphasia: Evidence from an auditory lexical decision task. Brain and Language, 17, 301–315.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bookheimer, S.Y., Zeffiro, T.A., Blaxton, T., Gaillard, W. & Theodore, W. (1995). Regional cerebral blood flow during object naming and word reading. Human Brain Mapping, 3, 93–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, D.C., Garrett, M.F. & Zurif, E.B. (1980). Syntactic deficits in Broca’s aphasia. In D. Caplan (ed.), Biological studies of mental processes, Cambridge, MA: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg, V. & Pulvermüller, F. (1992). Entwurf einer neurologischen Theorie der Sprache. Naturwissenschaft, 79, 103–117.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R.L., Petersen, S.E., Ojemann, J.G., Miezin, F.M., Squire, L.R. & Raichle, M.E. (1995). Functional anatomical studies of explicit and implicit memory retrieval tasks. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 12–29.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (1997). Imaging Cognition: An empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, D. (1992). Language: Structure, processing, and disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, D. & Aydelott-Utman, J. (1994). Selective acoustic phonetic impairment and lexical access in an aphasic patient. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 95, 512–517.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A. & Zurif, E. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia Brain and Language, 3, 572–582.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chertkow, H., Bub, D. & Caplan, D. (in press). Stages of semantic memory: Evidence from dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chwilla, D.J., Brown, C. & Hagoort, P. (1995). The N400 as a function of the level of processing. Psychophysiology, 32, 274–285.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In B. Underwood (ed.), Strategies of Information processing. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Patterson, J. & Marshall, J.C. (1980). Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulson, S., King, J. & Kutas, M. (1995). The late show: The syntactic positive shift meets the late positive component. Paper presented at the Eight Annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing. Tucson, Arizona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Bettinardi, V., Tadary, B., Woods, R., Mazziotta, J.C. & Fazio, F. (1994). Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography. Nature, 371, 600–602.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dejerne, J. (1891). Sur un cas de cecité verbale avec agraphie, suivi d’autopsie. Compte rendu des séances de la société de biologie, 3, 197–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dejeme, J. (1892). Contribution à l’étude anatomoclinique et clinique des differentes variétés de cecité verbale. Memories de la Société de Biologie, 4, 61–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Démonet, J.F., Chollet, F., Ramsay, S., Cardebat, D., Nespoulous, J.L., Wise, R., Rascol, A. & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1992). The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. Brain, 115, 1753–1768.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Démonet, J-F., Wise, R. & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1993). Language functions explored in normal subjects by positron emission tomography: A critical review. Human Brain Mapping, 1, 39–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Démonet, J.-F., Price, C., Wise, R. & Frackowiak, R. (1994). Differential activation of right and left posterior sylvian regions by semantic and phonological tasks: A positron-emission tomography study in normal human subjects. Neuroscience Letters, 182, 25–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P.D., Siqueland, E.R., Juszyk, P. & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A.W. (1982). Spelling and writing (and reading and speaking). In A.W. Ellis (ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive function, London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A.W. & Young, A.W. (1988). Human Cognitive Neuropsychology. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eulitz, C., Maeß, B., Pantev, Chr., Friederici, A.D., Feige, B. & Elbert, T. (1996). Oscillatory neuromagnetic activity induced by language and non-language stimuli. Cognitive Brain Research, 4, 2748–2755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiez, J.A. (1997). Phonology, semantics, and the role of the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 79–83.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frauenfelder, U.H. & Tyler, L.K. (1987). The process of spoken word recognition: An introduction. Cognition, 25, 1–20.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1978). On Comprehending Sentences: Syntactic Parsing Strategies. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Connecticut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1987a). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1987b). Theories of sentence processing. In J. Garfield (ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural-language processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. (1983). Aphasics’ perception of words in sentential context: Some real-time processing evidence. Neuropsychologia, 21, 351–358.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedend, A.D. (1984). Neuropsychologie der Sprache. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. (1985). Levels of processing and vocabulary types: Evidence from on-line comprehension in normals and agrammatics. Cognition, 19, 133–166.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. (1988). Agrammatic comprehension: Picture of a computational mismatch. Aphasiology, 2 (3/4), 279–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. (1995). The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data. Brain and Language, 50, 259–281.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Hahne, A. & Mecklinger, A. (1996). The temporal structure of syntactic parsing: Early and late effects elicited by syntactic anomalies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 5, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. & Kilborn, K. (1989). Temporal constraints on language processing: Syntactic priming in Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 262–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Syntactic parsing as revealed by brain responses: First-pass and second-pass parsing processes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 1, 157–176.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Pfeifer, E. & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: Effects of semantic morphological and syntactic violations. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 183–192.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K.J., Price, C.J., Pletcher, P., Moore, C., Frackowiak, R.S. & Dolan, R. (1996). The trouble with cognitive substraction. NeuroImage, 4, 97–104.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C.D., Friston, K.J., Liddle, P.F. & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1991). A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia, 29, 1137–1148.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K. (1913). Über die Störungen der Grammatik bei Hirnkrankheiten. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 34, 540–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H. & Baker, E. (1976). Semantic field, naming, and auditory comprehension in aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 359–374.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, B. & Caramazza, A. (1982). Lexical decision for open- and closed-class words: Failure to replicate differential frequency sensitivity. Brain and Language, 15, 143–160.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gorrell, P. (1995). Syntax and parsing. Cambridge, Engl.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunter, T.C., Stowe, L.A. & Mulder, G. (in press). When syntax meets semantics. Psychophysiology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haarmann, H.J. & Kolk, H.H.J. (1991). Syntactic priming in Broca’s aphasics: Evidence for slow activation. Aphasiology, 5, 247–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haarmann, H.J. & Kolk, H.H.J. (1994). On-line sensitivity to subject-verb agreement violations in Broca’s aphasics: The role of syntactic complexity and time. Brain and Language, 46, 493–516.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P. (1993). Impairments of lexical-semantic processing in aphasia: Evidence from the processing of lexical ambiguities. Brain and Language, 45, 189–232.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hahne, A. & Friedend, A.D. (1997). Language comprehension: The brain dissociates early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K.M. & Scholes, R.J. (1976). The nature of comprehension errors in Brocas conduction, and Wernickes aphasies. Cortex, 12, 258–265.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, T. (1985). Allgemeine Sprachpsychologie. München: Urban & Schwarzenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holcomb, P.J. & Neville, H.J. (1990). Semantic priming in visual and auditory lexical decision: A between modality comparison. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 281–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holcomb, P.J. & Neville, H.J. (1991). Natural speech processing: An analysis using event-related brain potentials. Psychobiology, 19, 286–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, D., Patterson, K., Wise, R., Brown, W.D., Friston, K., Weiller, D. & Frackowiak, R. (1992). The cortical localization of the lexicons. Positron emission tomography evidence. Brain, 115, 1769–1782.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G.W. & Evett, L.J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexieal routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 689–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1987). On beyond zebra: The relation of linguistic and visual information. Cognition, 26, 89–114.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, J.M., Mcintosh, A.R., Kapur, S., Tulving, E. & Houle, S. (1977). Cognitive substraction may not add up: The interaction between semantic processing and response mode. NeuroImage, 5, 229–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M.A., Carpenter, P.A., Keller, T.A., Eddy, W.F. & Thulborn, K.R. (1996). Brain Activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114–116.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, S., Craik, F.I.M., Tulving, E., Wilson, A.A., Houle, S. & Brown, G. (1994). Neuroanatomical correlates of encoding in episodic memory: Levels of processing effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 91, 2008–2011.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kaukoranta, E., Hari, R. & Lounasma, O.V. (1987). Responses of the human auditory cortex to vowel onset after fricative consonants. Experimental Brain Research, 69, 19–23.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A. (1979). Aphasia and associated disorders: Taxonomy, localization and recovery. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A. (1983). Localization of lesions in Wernicke’s aphasia. In A. Kertesz (ed.), Localization in neuropsychology. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, J. & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word and clause related ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 378–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. & Warrington, E.K. (1962). A variety of reading disability associated with right hemisphere lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 25, 339–344.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D.H. (1979). Speech perception: A model of acoustic-phonetic analysis and lexical access. In R.A. Cole (ed.), Perception and production of fluent speech. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D.H. (1986). The problem of variability in speech recognition and in models of speech perception. In J. Perkell & D. Klatt (eds.), Invariance and variability in speech processes Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D.H. (1989). Review of selected models of speech perception. In W. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, R. & Kutas, M. (1993). Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 573–633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolk, H.H.J. & Blomert, L. (1985). On the Bradley-hypothesis concerning agrammatism: The non-word interference effect. Brain and Language, 21, 47–67.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kolk, H.H. & van Grunsven, J.J.F. (1985). Agrammatism as a variable phenomenon. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 347–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S.M. (1994). On cognitive neuroscience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 297–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kounios, J. & Holcomb, P.J. (1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 804–823.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuriki, S. & Murase, M. (1989). Neuromagnetic study of the auditory responses in right and left hemispheres of the human brain evoked by pure tones and speech sounds. Experimental Brain Research, 77, 127–134.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S.A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S.A.(1983). Event-related potentials to grammatical errors and semantic anomalies. Memory and Cognition, 11, 539–550.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., Lindamood, T. & Hillyard, S.A. (1984). Word expectancy and event-related brain potentials during sentence processing. In S. Kornblum & J. Requin (eds.), Preparatory states and processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauter, J.L., Herscovitch, P., Formby, C. & Raichle, M.E. (1985). Tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex revealed by positron emission tomography. Hearing Research, 20, 199–205.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.M., Cooper, F.S., Shankweiler, D.P. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.M. & Mattingly, I.G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Linebarger, M.C., Schwartz, M.E & Saffran, E.M. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition, 13, 361–392.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M.C., Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 56–98.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mangun, G.R., Hillyard, S.A. & Luck, S.J. (1993). Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention. In D. Meyer & S. Kornblum (eds.), Attention and Performance, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. & Tyler, L.K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition, 8, 1–71.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J.C. & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2 (3), 175–199.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Haxby, J.V., Lalonde, F.M., Wiggs, C.L. & Ungerleider, L.G. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of colour and knowledge of action. Science, 270, 102–105.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C.L., Ungerleider, L.G. & Haxby, J.V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649–652.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, G. & Wood, C.C. (1985). Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: An ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 62, 203–208.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., St. John, M. & Taraban, R. (1989). An interaction model of context effects in letter perception: Part I. An account of basic findings. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 287–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mecklinger, A. & Meinshausen, R.-M. (in press). Recognition memory for object form and location: An event-related potential study. Memory & Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K. & Friedend, A.D. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory and Cognition, 23, 477–494.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miceli, G., Gainotti, G., Caltagirone, C. & Masullo, C. (1980). Some aspects of phonological impairment in aphasia. Brain and Language, 11, 159–169.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miceli, G., Mazzucchi, A., Menn, L. & Goodglass, H. (1983). Contrasting cases of Italian agrammatic aphasia without comprehension disorder. Brain and Language, 19, 65–97.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Milberg, W. & Blumstein, S.E.(1981). Lexical decision and aphasia: Evidence for semantic processing. Brain and Language, 14, 371–385.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Milberg, W., Blumstein, S.E. & Dworetzky, B. (1987). Processing lexical ambiguities. Brain and Language, 31, 138–150.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. & Patterson, K.E. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation, or, an attempt at a new interpretation. In M. Coltheart, K.E. Patterson & J.C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Münte, T.F., Heinze, H.I. & Mangun, G.R. (1993). Dissociation of brain activity related to syntactic and semantic aspects of language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 335–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen, R. & Picton, T.W. (1987). The N1 ware of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology, 24, 375–425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H.J., Nicol, J., Barss, A., Forster, K.I. & Garrett, M.F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H.J., Mills, D.L. & Lawson, D.S. (1992). Fractionating language: Different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 244–258.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nobre, A.C., Allison, T. & McCarthy, G. (1994). Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe. Nature, 372, 260–263.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nobre, A.C. & McCarthy, G. (1994). Language-related ERPs: Scalp distribution and modulation by word type and semantic priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 233–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nobre, A.C. & McCarthy, G. (1995). Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of the word type and semantic priming. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15 (2), 1090–1098.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Event-related potentials and syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P.J. (1993). Event-related potentials and syntactic anomaly: Evidence of anomaly detection during the perception of continuous speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 413–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J. & Swinney, D. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 786–803.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. & Mobley, L.A. (1995). Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 739–773.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pantev, C., Hoke, K., Lehnertz, K. & Lütkenhöner, B. (1989). Neuromagnetic evidence of an amplitopic organization of the human auditory cortex. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 225–231.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K.E. (1980). Derivational errors. In M. Coltheart, K.E. Patterson & J.C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K.E. & Kay, J. (1982). Letter-by-letter reading: Psychological descriptions of a neurological syndrome. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 411–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penke, M., Weyerts, H., Gross, M., Zander, E., Miinte, T.F. & Clahsen, H. (1997). How the brain processes complex words: An ERP-study of German verb inflections. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, 37–52.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Posner, M.I., Mintum, M. & Raichle, M.E. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature, 331, 585–589.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Posner, M.I., Mintum, M. & Raichle, M.E. (1989). Positron emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 153–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Snyder, A.Z. & Raichle, M.E. (1990). Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. Science, 249, 1041–1044.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pick, A. (1913). Die agrammatischen Sprachstörungen. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poeppel, D., Yellin, E., Phillips, C., Roberts, T.P.L., Rowley, H.A., Wexler, K. & Marantz, A. (1996). Task-induced asymmetry of the auditory evoked M 100 neuromagnetic field elicited by speech sounds. Cognitive Brain Research, 4, 231–242.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., Peterson, S.E., Fox, P.T. & Raichle, M.E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science, 240, 1627–1632.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Price, C., Wise, R., Ransay, S., Friston, K., Howard, D., Patterson, K. & Frackowiak, R. (1992). Regional response within the human auditory cortex when listening to words. Neuroscience Letters, 146, 179–182.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Price, C.J., Wise, R.J.S., Watson, J.D.G., Patterson, K., Howard, D. & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1994). Brain activity during reading: The effects of exposure duration and task. Brain, 117, 1255–1269.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F., Lutzenberger, W. & Birbaumer, N. (1995). Electrocortical distinction of vocabulary types. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 94, 357–370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M.E., Fiez, J.A., Videen, T.O., MacLeod, A.M., Pardo, J.V., Fox, P.T. & Petersen, S.E. (1994). Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. Cerebral Cortex, 4 (1), 8–26.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosier, F., Friedend, A.D., Pütz, P. & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials while encountering semantic and syntactic constraint violations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 345–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S.S. & Rubenstein, M.A. (1971). Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 645–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffian, E.M. & Schwartz, M.E (1994). Of cabbages and things: Semantic memory from a neuropsychological perspective — A tutorial view. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (eds.), Attention and performance XV Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G. & Job, R. (1988). The oyster with four legs: A neuropsychological study on the interaction of visual and semantic information. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 105–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sasanuma, S., Akio, K. & Kubota, M. (1990). Agrammatism in Japanese: Two case studies. In L. Menn & L. Obler (eds.), Agrammatic Aphasia. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherg, M. & von Cramon, D. (1986). Evoked dipole source potentials in the human auditory cortex. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 65, 344–360.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scherg, M. (1990). Fundamentals of dipole source potential analysis. In F. Grandori, M. Hoke & G.L. Romani (eds.), Auditory evoked magnetic fields and electric potentials. Basel: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder, R. & Flores d’Arcais, G.B. (1989). Psycholinguistic issues in the lexical representation of meaning. In W.D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segui, J., Mehler, J., Frauenfelder, U. & Morton, J. (1982). Word frequency effect and lexical access. Neuropsychologia, 20, 615–627.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S., Waters, G.S., Barnes, M.A. & Tanenhaus, M.K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 383–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. & Warrington, E.K. (1980). Single and multiple component central dyslexic syndromes. In M. Coltheart, K.E. Patterson & J.C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., Warrington, E.K. & McCarthy, R. (1983). Reading without semantics. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 111–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Identification of consonants and vowels presented to left and right ears. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 59–63.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Silveri, M.C. & Gainotti, G.B. (1987). Interaction between vision and language in category specific semantic impairment for living things. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 677–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N. & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language, 52, 452–473.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D.A., Zurif, E.B. & Cutler, A. (1980). Effects of sentential stress and word class upon comprehension in Broca’s aphasics. Brain and Language, 10, 132–144.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., Markowitsch, H.J., Kapur, S., Habib, R. & Houle, S. (1994). Novelty encoding networks in the human brain: Positron emission tomography data. NeuroReport, 5, 2525–2528.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van Petten, C. (1995). Words and Sentences: Event-related brain potential measures. Psychophysiology, 32, 511–525.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Petten, C. & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory and Cognition, 18 (4), 380–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Stockert, T.R. & Bader, L. (1976). Some relations of grammar and lexicon in aphasia. Cortex, 12, 49–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E.K. & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–853.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, P., Caramazza, A. & Zurif, E.B. (1978). Naming in aphasia: Interacting effects of form and function. Brain and Language, 6, 63–74.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, R., Chollet, F., Hadar, U., Friston, K., Hoffner, E. & Frackowiak, R. (1991). Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. Brain, 114, 1803–1817.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfeck, B. (1988). Grammatically judgement and sentence comprehension in agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 72–81.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre, R.J., Meyer, E., Gjedde, A. & Evans, A.C. (1996). PET studies of phonetic processing of speech: Review, replication, and reanalysis. In M. Raichle & P.S. Goldman-Rakic (eds.), Special Issue: Cortical imaging — microscope of the mind, Cerebral Cortex, 6, 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre, R.J., Evans, A.C., Meyer, E. & Gjedde, A. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science, 256, 846–849.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zurif, E.B., Caramazza, A. & Myerson, R. (1972). Grammatical judgements of agrammatic aphasics. Neuropsychologia, 10, 405–417.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zurif, E.B., Caramazza, A., Myerson, R. & Galvin, J. (1974). Semantic feature representation for normal and aphasic language. Brain and Language, 1, 167–187.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Friederici, A.D. (1998). The Neurobiology of Language Comprehension. In: Language Comprehension: A Biological Perspective. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-97734-3_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-97734-3_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-97736-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-97734-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics