Skip to main content

The Architecture of the Mental Lexicon

  • Chapter

Abstract

The original subject of research on the mental lexicon is the subjective representation of word meanings. Just as in linguistics words are allocated their meanings in the lexicon, words were allocated a mental representation in the psychology of the 1970’s. This position could, however, not be maintained. Assumptions regarding representation have become increasingly differentiated, so that now the mental lexicon must be understood as a part of the whole cognitive architecture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1978). Arguments concerning representations for mental imagery. Psychological Review, 85, 249–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1996). Kognitive Psychologic Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bäckman, L. & Nilsson, L.-G. (1985). Prerequisites for lack of age differences in memory performance. Experimental Aging Research, 11, 67–73.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, J.R., Bransford, J.D., Franks, J.J., McCarrell, N.S. & Nitsch, K. (1974). Comprehension and semantic flexibility. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 471–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L.W. (1982). Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. Memory & Cognition, 10, 82–93.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartram, D. J. (1976). Levels of coding in picture-picture comparison tasks. Memory & Cognition, 4, 593–602.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J. N., Smith, M. C. & MacLeod, C. M. (1989). Auditory and visual word-stem completion: Separating data-driven and conceptually driven processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 439–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. & Cooper, E. E. (1991). Priming contour-deleted images: Evidence for intermediate representations in visual object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 393–419.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. & Cooper, E. E. (1992). Size invariance in visual object priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 121–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierwisch, M. & Schreuder, R. (1992). From concepts to lexical items. Cognition, 42, 23–60.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boles, D. B. (1983). Dissociated imageability, concreteness, and familiarity in lateralized word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 11, 511–519.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, J. S. & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Implicit memory and test awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 404–416.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. W. & McNeill, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue”-phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 325–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bub, D. & Kertesz, A. (1982a). Evidence for lexicographic processing in a patient with preserved written over oral single word naming. Brain, 105, 697–717.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bub, D. & Kertesz, A. (1982b). Deep agraphia. Brain and Language, 18, 128–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1934). Sprachtheorie: Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache. Jena: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., Berndt, R. S. & Basili, A. G. (1983). Autobiographical memory and perceptual learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and perceptual identification. Memory and Cognition, 13, 273–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., Hillis, A., Leek, E. C. & Miozzo, M. (1994). The organization of lexical knowledge in the brain: Evidence from category- and modality-specific deficits. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman, (eds.), Mapping the Mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caroll, M., Byrne, B. & Kirsner, K. (1985). Autobiographical memory and perceptual learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and perceptual identification. Memory & Cognition, 13, 273–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Challis, B. H. & Brodbeck, D. R. (1992). Level of processing affects priming in word fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 595–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H.-C., Cheung, H. & Lau, S. (in press). Examining and reexamining the structure of Chinese-English bilingual memory. Psychological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. L. (1981). On the generality of some memory laws. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 267–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. L. (1983). The effect of encoding variables on the free recall of words and action events. Memory and Cognition, 11, 575–582.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. L. (1989). Memory for action events: The power of enactment. Educational Psychological Review, 1, 57–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M. & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M. & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M. & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M. & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 155–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dannenbring, G. L. & Briand, K. (1982). Semantic priming and the word repetition effect in a lexical decision task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 435–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deese, J. (1970). Psycholinguistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Groot, A. M. B., Danneburg, L. & van Hell, J. G. (1994). Forward and backward word translation. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 600–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Den Heyer, K., Goring, A. & Dannenbring, G. L. (1985). Semantic priming and word repetition: The two effects are additive. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 699–716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M., Engelkamp, J. & Mohr, G. (1991). Memory of imagined actions: Imagining oneself or another person. Psychological Research, 53, 246–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Renzi, E. & Lucchelli, F. (1994). Are semantic systems separately represented in the brain? The case of living category impairment. Cortex, 30, 3–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dhawan, M. & Pellegrino, J. W. (1977). Acoustic and semantic interference effects in words and pictures. Memory & Cognition, 5, 340–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durso, F. T. & Johnson, M. K. (1979). Facilitation in naming and categorizing repeated pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 449–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. W. & Young, A. W. (1989). Human cognitive neuropsychology. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. W. (1983). Syndromes, slips and structures. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 36, 372–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1973). Semantische Struktur und die Verarbeitung von Sätzen. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1985). Die Repräsentation der Wortbedeutung. In C. Schwarze & D. Wunderlich (eds.), Handbuch der Lexikologie. Königstein/Ts.: Athenäum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1987). Arguments for a visual memory system. In J. Engelkamp, K. Lorenz & B. Sandig (eds.), Wissensrepräsentation und Wissensaustausch. St. Ingbert: Röhrig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1990). Das menschliche Gedächtnis. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1991a). Memory of action events: Some implications for memory theory and for imagery. In C. Cornoldi & M. McDaniel (eds.), Imagery and cognition. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1991b). Imagery and enactment in paired-associate learning. In R. H. Logie & M. Denis (eds.), Mental images in human cognition. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1994). Mentale Repräsentationen im Kontext verschiedener Aufgaben. In H.-J. Kornadt, J. Grabowski & R. Mangold-Allwinn (eds.), Sprache und Kognition — Perspektiven moderner Sprachpsychologie. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1995). Visual imagery and enactment of actions in memory. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 227–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1997a). Das Erinnern eigener Handlungen. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp (1997b). Memory for to-be-performed tasks versus memory for performed tasks. Memory and Cognition, 25, 117–124.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. & Krumnacker, H. (1980). Imaginale und motorische Prozesse beim Behalten verbalen Materials. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 27, 511–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. & Mohr, G. (1986). Legitimation und Bereitschaft bei der Rezeption von Aufforderungen. Sprache und Kognition, 3, 127–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J., Mohr, G. & Mohr, M. (1985). Zur Rezeption von Aufforderungen. Sprache und Kognition, 4, 65–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. & Wippich, W. (1995). Current issues in implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Research, 57, 143–155.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp J. & Zimmer, H. D. (1983) Zum Einfluß von Wahrnehmen und Tun auf das Behalten von Verb-Objekt-Phrasen. Sprache & Kognition, 2, 117–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. & Zimmer, H. D. (1994a). The human memory. A multi-modal approach. Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. & Zimmer, H. D. (1994b). Motor similarity in subject-performed tasks. Psychological Research, 57, 47–53.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. & Zimmer, H. D. (1995). Similarity of movement in recognition of self-performed tasks. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 241–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J., Zimmer, H. D. & Biegelmann, U. (1993). Bizarreness effects in verbal tasks and subject-performed tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 393–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J., Zimmer, H. D. & Kurbjuweit, A. (1995). Verb frequency and enactment in implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Research, 57, 242–249.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J., Zimmer, H. D., Mohr, G. & Sellen, O. (1994). Memory of self-performed tasks: Self-performing during recognition. Memory & Cognition, 22, 34–39.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flores d’Arcais, G. B., Schreuder, R. & Glazenborg, G. (1985). Semantic activation during recognition of referential words. Psychological Research, 47, 39–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In R. J. Wales & E. C. T. Walker (eds.), New Approaches to language mechanisms. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. (1989). Basic issues in lexical processing. In W. D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 680–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, J. M., Gregg, V. H. & Hampton, J. A. (1988). Word frequency and generation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 14, 687–693.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, W. R. & Glaser, M. O. (1989). Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 13–42.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glass, A. L., Holyoak, K. J. & Santa, J. L. (1979). Cognition. London: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorman, A. M. (1961). Recognition memory for nouns as a function of abstractness and frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 23–29.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P. & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosser, Ch., Pobel, R., Mangold-Allwinn, R. & Herrmann, Th. (1989). Determinanten des Allgemeinheitsgrades von Objektbenennungen (Arbeiten der Forschergruppe “Sprechen und Sprachverstehen im sozialen Kontext” Heidelberg/ Mannheim, Bericht Nr. 24). Universität Mannheim: Lehrstuhl Psychologie III.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, T. A. (1995). The psychology of language: From data to theory. Erlbaum: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). Organization of Behavior. New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helstrup, T. (1987). One, two or three memories? A problem-solving approach to memory for performed acts. Acta Psychologica, 66, 37–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemphill, R. E. & Stengel, E. (1940). A study on pure word deafness. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 3, 251–262.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th. (1982). Sprechen und Situation. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th. (1985). Allgemeine Sprachpsychologie. Grundlagen und Probleme. München: Urban & Schwarzenberg. (2. Aufl.- Weinheim: Beltz, Psychologie-Verlags-Union, 1994.).

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th. (1994). Psychologie ohne “Bedeutung”? Zur Wort-Konzept-Relation in der Psychologie. Sprache & Kognition, 13, 126–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th. & Deutsch, W. (1976). Psychologie der Objektbenennung. (Studien zur Sprachpsychologie 5). Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th. & Grabowski, J. (1994). Sprechen: Psychologie der Sprachproduktion. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th., Grabowski, J., Schweizer, K. & Graf, R. (1996). Die mentale Repräsentation von Konzepten, Wörtern und Figuren. In J. Grabowski, G. Harras & T. Herrmann (eds.), Bedeutung, Konzepte, Bedeutungskonzepte: Theorien und Anwendung in Linguistik und Psychologie. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, H. (1983). Bewegungslernen. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, H. (1990). Psychomotorik. In H. Spada (ed.), Allgemeine Psychologie. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hier, D. B. & Mohr, J. P. (1977). Incongruous oral and written naming: Evidence for a subdivision of the syndrome of Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 4, 115–126.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, D. (1976). Recognition of verbs, abstract nouns, and concrete nouns from the left and right visual half-fields. Neuropsychologia, 14, 211–216.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J. (1996). Die Genese von Begriffen, Bedeutungen und Wörtern. In J. Grabowski, G. Harras & T. Herrmann (eds.), Bedeutung, Konzepte, Bedeutungskonzepte: Theorien und Anwendung in Linguistik und Psychologie. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homa, D. & Viera, C. (1988). Long-term memory for pictures under conditions of thematically related foils. Memory & Cognition, 16, 411–421.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hörmann, H. (1967). Psychologie der Sprache. Heidelberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hörmann, H. (1978). Meinen und Verstehen. Grundzüge einer psychologischen Semantik. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. L., Rabinowitz, F. M. & Grant, M. J. (1993). On measuring (in-)dependence of cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100, 737–747.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. & Bruce, V. (1989). Visual cognition. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. & Riddoch, M. J. (1987a). To see but not to see: A case study of visual agnosia. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. & Riddoch, M. J. (1987b). Visual object processing: A cognitive neuropsychological approach. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., Evett, J. L., Quinlan, P. T. & Besner, D. (1987). Orthographic priming: Qualitative differences between priming from identified and unidentified primes. In M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and Performance XII. The psychology of reading. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, A. & Morton, J. (1984). Facilitation of auditory word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 12, 568–574.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N., Herrmann, D. J. & Chaffin, R. (1984). Only connections: A critique of semantic networks. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 292–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior (1984). Special edition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, J. & Ellis, A. W. (1987). A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia: Implications for psychological models of word retrieval. Brain, 110, 613–629.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelter, S. (1994). Kognitive Semantik und Aphasieforschung. Zur Unterscheidung zwischen Bedeutungen und Konzepten. In M. Schwarz (ed.), Kognitive Semantik/ Cognitive Semantics. Ergebnisse, Probleme, Perspektiven (Tübinger Beiträge zur Linguistik, 395). Tübingen: Narr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer, M., Barattelli, S. & Mangold-Allwinn, R. (1993). Kognition und Kommunikation: Ein integrativer Ansatz zur multiplen Determination der lexikalischen Spezifität der Objektklassenbezeichnung (Arbeiten aus dem Sonderforschungsbereich 245 “Sprache und Situation” Heidelberg/Mannheim, Bericht Nr. 51). Universität Mannheim: Lehrstuhl Psychologie III.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer, M., Barattelli, S. Sc., Mangold-Allwinn, R. (1995). Vom generierten Konzept zum enkodierten Wort: Zur Determination der lexikalischen Spezifität von Objektbenennungen. Linguistische Berichte, 159, 381–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1972). Notes on the structure of semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (eds.), Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K., Dunn, J. C. & Standen, P. (1989). Domain-specific resources in word recognition. In S. Levandowsky, J. C. Dunn & K. Kirsner (eds.), Implicit memory. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K., Milech, D. & Stunpfel, V. (1986). Word and picture identification: Is representational parsimony possible? Memory & Cognition, 14, 398–408.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klix, F. (1980). Die allgemeine Psychologie und die Erforschung kognitiver Prozesse. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 188, 117–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klix, F. (1985). Über Basisprozesse für geistige Dispositionen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 193, 27–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1981). The medium and the message in mental imagery: A theory. Psychological Review, 88, 46–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F. & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. N., Calvando, R. & Popovics, A. (1982). Language in the absence of inner speech. Neuropsychologia, 20, 391–409.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liepmann, H. (1905/8). Drei Aufsätze aus dem Apraxie gebiet. Berlin: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H. (1986). Visuo-spatial processing in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 229–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. L. (1982). Priming and search processes in semantic memory: A test of three models of spreading activation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 468–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupker, S. J. (1988). Picture naming: An investigation of the nature of categorical priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 444–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1977). Introduction to Theoretical Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madigan, S. (1983). Picture memory. In J. C. Yuille (ed.), Imagery, memory and cognition. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., Goodman, G. O. & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. L. (1982). The word-frequency paradox in recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10, 33–42.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mangold-Allwinn, R. (1993). Flexible Konzepte: Experimente, Modelle, Simulationen. Frankfurt/M.: Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangold-Allwinn, R., Barattelli, S., Kiefer, M. & Koelbing, H.-G. (1995). Wörter für Dinge. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, D. F. & Isaak, A. R. (1995). Topographical distribution of EEG activity accompanying visual and motor imagery in vivid and non-vivid images. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 271–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R. C. & Jensen, C. R. (1988). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: A failure to replicate. Memory & Cognition, 16, 505–521.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive neuropsychology. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. (1987). The case for interactions in language processing. In M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and Performance XII: The psychology of reading. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. & Elman, J. O. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–86.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Part 1: An Account of Basic Findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P. & Healy, A. F. (1988). Semantic, phonological, and mediated priming in reading and lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 398–409.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. E. & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mohr, G. (1989). Auffordern und Begründen. Die Integration verstehens- und entscheidungsrelevanter Informationen. Saarbrücken: Dadder.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohr, G. & Engelkamp, J. (1996). Neuropsychologische und allgemeinpsychologische Evidenzen für die Modularität des Sprachsystems. In A. Gather & H. Werner (eds.), Semiotische Prozesse und natürliche Sprache. Stuttgart: Steiner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1979). Facilitation in word recognition: Experiments causing change in the logogen model. In P. A. Kolers, M. E. Wrolstad & H. Bouma (eds.), Processing of visible language I. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J & Patterson, K.E. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation or an attempt at a new interpretation. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson & J. C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees. The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L. (1979). Remembering pictures and words: Appearance, significance and name. In L. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik (eds.), Levels of processing in human memory. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L. & Brooks, D. H. (1973). Independence of phonetic and imaginai features. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L. & Reed, V. (1976). On the nature of pictorial encoding: A levels-of-processing analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 49–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., Reed, V. & Walling, J. R. (1976). Pictorial superiority effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 523–528.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. (1992). Feature analysis and the role of similarity in preattentive vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 355–375.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1969). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychological Review, 76, 241–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehard & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., Siegel, A. W. & Dhawan, M. (1975). Short-term retention of pictures and words: Evidence for dual coding systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 104, 95–102.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M. & Raichle, M. E. (1989). Positron emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1 (2), 153–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pobel, R. (1991). Objektrepräsentation und Objektbenennung. Situative Einflüsse auf die Wortwahl beim Benennen von Gegenständen. Regensburg: Roderer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potter, M. C. & Faulconer, B. A. (1975). Time to understand pictures and words. Nature, 253, 437–438.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Potter, M. C., So, K. F., von Eckart, B. & Feldman, I. B. (1984). Lexical and conceptual representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 23–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1983). Wahrnehmung und Tätigkeitssteuerung. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 88, 16–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Coltheart, M. & Funnell, E. (1988). Semantic systems or system? Neuropsychological evidence re-examined. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory. Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. & Weldon, M. S. (1987). Reversing the picture superiority effect. In M. McDaniel & M. Pressley (eds.), Imagery and related mnemonic processes. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L. & Riegler, G. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 1251–1269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosati, G. & de Bastiani, P. (1979). Pure agraphia: A discrete form of aphasia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 42, 266–269.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1991). Human motor control. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothi, L. J. G. & Heilman, K. M. (1996). Liepmann (1900 and 1905): A definition of apraxia and a model of praxis. In C. Code, C.-W. Walesch, Y. Joanette & A. R. Lecours (eds.), Classic cases in Neuropsychology. Hove: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Lindsay, P. H. & Norman, D. A. (1972). A process model of long term memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (eds.). Organization of memory. New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saltz, E. & Donnenwerth-Nolan, S. (1981). Does motoric imagery facilitate memory for sentences? A selective interference test. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 20, 322–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., Masterson, J. & Job, R. (1987). Direct-route reading and the locus of lexical decision. In M. Coltheart, G. Sartori & R. Job (eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1990). Perceptual representation systems and implicit memory: Toward a resolution of the multiple memory systems debate. In A. Diamond (ed.), Development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. & Church, B. (1992). Auditory priming: Implicit and explicit memory for words and voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 915–930.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Cooper, L. A. & Delany, S. M. (1990). Implicit memory for unfamiliar objects depends on access to structural descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 5–24.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Delany, S. M. & Merikle, E. P. (1990). Priming of nonverbal information and the nature of implicit memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 83–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1972). Conceptual dependency: A theory of natural language understanding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 552–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1975). Conceptual information processing. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, N. F., Saffran, E. M. & Marin, O. S. M. (1980). Fractionating the reading process in dementia: Evidence for word-specific print-to-sound-associations. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson & J. C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, M. (1992). Kognitive Semantiktheorie und neuropsychologist Realität. Repräsentationale und prozedurale Aspekte der sematischen Kompetenz. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanon, B. (1979). Lateralization effects in response to words and nonwords. Cortex, 15, 541–549.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. J. (1971). Psycholinguistics. London: Scott, Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. C. & Magee, L. E. (1980). Tracing the time course of picture-word processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 373–392.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G. (1984). Concepts and their surface representation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solso, R. L. (1979). Cognitive psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Janovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, M. (1996). Geist im Netz. Modelle für Lernen, Denken und Handeln. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strube, G. (1984). Assoziation. Der Prozeß des Erinnerns und die Struktur des Gedächtnisses. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M. & Forster, K. J. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior, 15, 607–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman A. & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15–48.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, R. E. (1977). Time and spread of activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 458–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, R. E. & Morton, J. (1982). The effects of priming of picture recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 117–129.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K. & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106, 859–878.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, M. J., Peynircioglu, Z. F. & Brems, D. J. (1984). Pictorial rehearsal. Memory & Cognition, 12, 553–557.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S. & Coyote, K. C. (1996). Failure to find the picture superiority effect in implicit conceptual memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 670–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, J. E. (1972). Encoding and memory for verbal and pictorial stimuli. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 242–252.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wippich, W., Mecklenbräucker, S., Weidmann, H. & Reichert, A. (1995). Priming-Effekte beim Bilderrätseln: Erste Lösungsvorschläge. Zeitschrift für experimentelle Psychologie, 42, 324–352.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. (1984). Blume oder Rose? Unterschiede in der visuellen Informationsverarbeitung bei Experten und Laien. Archiv für Psychologie, 136, 343–361.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. (1988). Formkonzepte und Bildmarken: Zwei verschiedene Repräsentationen für visuell-sensorische Merkmale? Sprache & Kognition, 7, 40–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. (1993). Modalitätsspezifische Systeme der Repräsentation und Verarbeitung von Informationen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 201, 203–235.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. (1996). Memory for spatial location and enactment. Psychologische Beiträge, 38, 404–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. & Biegelmann, U. E. (1990a). Die Herausbildung und Beurteilung von Basisbegriffen in einer artifiziellen Farbhierarchie (Arbeiten der Fachrichtung Psychologie, Bericht Nr. 151). Saarbrücken: Universität des Saarlandes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. & Engelkamp, J. (1985). An attempt to distinguish between cinematic and motor memory components. Acta Psychologica, 58, 81–106.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D., Engelkamp, J. & Sieloff, U. (1984). Motorische Gedächtniskomponenten als partiell unabhängige Komponenten des Engramms verbaler Handlungsbeschreibungen. Sprache & Kognition, 3, 70–85.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Engelkamp, J., Rummer, R. (1998). The Architecture of the Mental Lexicon. In: Language Comprehension: A Biological Perspective. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-97734-3_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-97734-3_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-97736-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-97734-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics