Skip to main content

Spoken Words in Sentence Contexts

  • Chapter

Abstract

Listeners sometimes have the impression that they know exactly which word a speaker is going to say next. However real such observations are, they are the exception rather than the rule. More than forty years of research has shown that recognizing spoken words is not a guessing game. Normally, word recognition is an extremely fast and efficient process that rarely reaches conscious awareness. Fluent speech is uttered at a rate of two to three words per second, and an adult language user has a mental lexicon in which the knowledge of about 30 000 to 50 000 words is stored (Aitchison, 1994). This implies that a listener has about one third of a second to select one word from this huge mental data base.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aitchisoo, J. (1994). Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon (2nd. ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B.J., Motley, M.T. & MacKay, D. (1975). Output editing for lexical status from artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 382–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bagley, W.C. (1990). The apperception of the spoken sentence: A study in the psychology of language. American Journal of Psychology, 12, 80–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A. & Chumbley, J.I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bard, E.G., Shillcock, R.C. & Altmann, G.T.M. (1988). The recognition of words after their acoustic offsets in spontaneous speech: Effects of subsequent context. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 395–408.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blank, M.A. & Foss, D.J. (1978). Semantic facilitation and lexical access during sentence processing. Memory and Cognition, 6, 644–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, Z.S. & Garnes, S. (1980). Misperceptions of fluent speech. In R.A. Cole (Ed.), Perception and production of fluent speech. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, D.C. & Forster, K.I. (1987). A reader’s view of listening. Cognition, 25, 103–134.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, H.S., Cowart, W. & Jablon, A.D. (1981). Effects of prior context upon the integration of lexical information during sentence processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 445–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, E.C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 25, 975–979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, R.A. (1973). Listening for mispronunciations: A measure of what we hear during speech. Perception and Psychophysics, 13, 153–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, R.A. & Jakimik, J. (1978). Understanding speech: How words are heard. In G. Underwood (ed.), Strategies of information processing. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, R.A. & Jakimik, J. (1980). A model of speech perception. In R.A. Cole (ed.), Perception and production of fluent speech. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, R.A. & Perfetti, C.A. (1980). Listening for mispronunciations in a children s story: The use of context by children and adults. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 297–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connine, C.M. (1990). Effects of sentence context and lexical knowledge in speech processing. In G.T.M. Altmann (ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connine, C.M. (1987). Constraints on interactive processes in auditory word recognition: The role of sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 527–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connine, C.M., Blasko, D.G. & Wang, J. (1994). Vertical similarity in spoken word recognition: Multiple lexical activation, individual differences, and the role of sentence context. Perception and Psychophysics, 56(6), 624–636.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, C. (1974). Context effects in sentence comprehension: A study of the subjective lexicon. Memory and Cognition, 2, 130–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cotton, S. & Grosjean, F. (1984). The gating paradigm: Successive or individual presentations? Perception and Psychophysics, 35, 41–48.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cowart, W. (1982). Autonomy and interaction in the language processing system: A reply to Marslen-Wilson and Tyler. Cognition, 12, 109–117.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. & McQueen, J.M. (1995). The recognition of lexical units in speech. In B. de Gelder and J. Morais (eds.), Speech and reading: A comparative approach. Hove, UK: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Segui, J. (1987). Phoneme identification and the lexicon. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 141–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. & Norris, D. (1979). Monitoring sentence comprehension. In W.E. Cooper & E.C.T. Walker (eds.), Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merril Garrett. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G.S. & Newman, J.E. (1980). Detecting phonemes in fluent speech. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 608–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, A. & Bentin, S. (1994). Attention mechanisms mediate the syntactic priming effect in auditory word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20(3), 595–607.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P.D. & Nygaard, L.C. (1992). Contextual coherence and attention in phoneme monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 375–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J.L. (1989). Connectionist approaches to acoustic/phonetic processing. In W.D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischler, I. & Bloom, P.A. (1979). Automatic and attentional processes in the effects of sentence contexts on word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In R.J. Wales & E. Walker (eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In W.E. Cooper & E.Walker (eds.), Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merril Garrett. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. (1981). Priming and the effects of sentence and lexical contexts on naming time: Evidence for autonomous lexical processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 465–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. (1989). Basic issues in lexical processing. In W.D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foss, D.J. & Blank, M.A. (1980). Identifying the speech codes. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 1–31.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Foss, D.J., Cirilo, R.K. & Blank, M.A. (1979). Semantic facilitation and lexical access during sentence processing: An investigation of individual differences. Memory and Cognition, 7, 346–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foss, D.J. & Gernsbacher, M.A. (1983). Cracking the dual code: Toward a unitary model of phoneme identification. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 609–632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foss, D.J. & Ross, J.R. (1983). Great expectations: Context effects during sentence processing. In G.B. Flores d’Arcais & R.J. Jarvella (eds.), The process of language understanding. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frauenfelder, U.H. (1996). Computational models of spoken word recognition. In T. Dijkstra & K. de Smedt (eds.), Computational psycholinguistics. London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frauenfelder, U.H. & Tyler, L.K. (1987). The process of spoken word recognition: An introduction. Cognition, 25, 1–20.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1987). Structure in auditory word recognition. Cognition, 25, 157–187.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedend, A.D. (1983). Aphasic perception of words in sentential context: Some real-time processing evidence. Neuropsychologia, 21(4), 351–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. (1987). Kognitive Strukturen des Sprachverstehens. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. (1997). Neurophysiological aspects of language processing. Clinical Neuroscience, 4(2), 64–72.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Hahne, A. & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Temporal structure of syntactic parsing: Early and late event-related potentials. Journal of experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22(5), 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. & Kilborn, K. (1989). Temporal constraints on language processing: Syntactic priming in Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 262–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D. & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Syntactic parsing as revealed by event-related potentials: First-pass and second-pass parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 157–176.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Pfeifer, E. & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related potentials during natural speech processing: Effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(3), 183–192.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Garnes, S. & Bond, Z.S. (1976). The relationship between semantic expectations and acoustic information. Phonologica, 3, 285–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnsey, S.M. (1993). Event-related potentials in the study of language: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 337–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glucksberg, S., Kreuz, R.J. & Rho, S.H. (1986). Context can constrain lexical access: Implications for models of language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 323–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, D.M. & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F. (1980). Spoken word recognition processes and the gating paradigm. Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 267–283.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F. (1985). The recognition of words after their acoustic offset: Evidence and implications. Perception and Psychophysics, 38, 299–310.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F. & Frauenfelder, U.H. (1997). A guide to spoken word recognition paradigms. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P., Brown, C. & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holcomb, P.J. & Neville, H.J. (1991). Natural speech processing: An analysis using event-related brain potentials. Psychobiology, 19, 286–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakimik, J.A. (1980). The interaction of sound and knowledge in word recognition from fluent speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie-Mellon University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L., Boyce, S., Goldstein, L. & Lukatela, G. (1987). Grammatical information effects in auditory word recognition. Cognition, 25(3), 235–263.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, G.W. (1980). Sentence frame contexts and lexical decision: Sentence-acceptability and word-relatedness effects. Memory and Cognition, 8, 336–344.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M. & Hilliard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M. & Van Petten, C. (1994). Psycholinguistics electrified: Event-related brain potential investigations. In M. A. Gernsbacher (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W.J.M. (1978). A survey of studies in sentence perception: 1970–1976. In W.J.M. Levelt & G.B. Flores d’Arcais (eds.), Studies in the perception of language. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, M.M. (1987). Frequency effects on the processing of ambiguous words in sentence context. Language and Speech, 30, 25–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1973). Speech shadowing and speech perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1984). Function and process in spoken word recognition. In H. Bouma & D.G. Bouwhuis (eds.), Attention and Performance X: Control of language processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1985). Speech shadowing and speech comprehension. Speech Communication, 4, 55–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 71–102.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1989). Access and integration: Projecting sound onto meaning. In W.D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Brown, C.M. & Tyler, L.K. (1988). Lexical representations in spoken language comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. & Tyler, L.K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition, 8, 1–71.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. & Tyler, L.K. (1983). Reply to Cowart. Cognition, 15, 227–235.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D. & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuos speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 29–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D.W. (1994). Psycholinguistic aspects of speech perception: Implications for research and theory. In M.A. Gernsbacher (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAllister, J.M. (1988). The use of context in auditory word recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 94–97.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCallum, W.C., Farmer, S.F. & Pocock, P.K. (1984). The effects of physical and semantic incongruities on auditory event-related potentials. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 59, 447–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L. & Elman, J.L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–86.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L. & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D.M. & Schvaneveldt, R.W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A. & Isard, S. (1963). Some perceptual consequences of linguistic rules. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 217–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J.L., Green, K. & Schermer, T.M. (1984). A distinction between the effects of sentential speaking rate and semantic congruency on word identification. Perception and Psychophysics, 36(4), 329–337.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., Heise, G.A. & Lichten, W. (1951). The intelligibility of speech as a function of the context and of the test materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 329–335.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. & Long, J. (1976). Effects of word transitional probability on phoneme identification. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 43–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, H.E. & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1993). Access to word meanings during spoken language comprehension: Effects of sentential semantic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19(6), 1254–1276.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, D. (1986). Word recognition: Context effects without priming. Cognition, 22, 93–136.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, D. (1987). Strategic control of sentence context in a naming task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 253–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oden, G.C. & Spira, J.L. (1983). Influence of context on the activation and selection of ambiguous word senses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 51–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onifer, W. & Swinney, D.A. (1981). Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: Effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias. Memory & Cognition, 9, 225–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Seaghdha, P.G. (1989). The dependence of lexical relatedness effects on syntactic correctness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 73–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. (1993). Event-related brain potentials and syntactic anomaly: Evidence of anomaly detection during the perception of continuous speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 413–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. (1995). ERPs and language comprehension. In M.D. Rugg & M.G.H. Coles (eds.), Electrophysiology of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ottevanger, I.B. (1986). Speech processing at the level of word recognition and the influence of sentence context. Doctoral dissertation, University of Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pisoni, D.B. & Luce, P.A. (1987). Acoustic-phonetic representations in word recognition. Cognition, 25, 21–52.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. & McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of priming in memory. Psychological Review, 95, 385–408.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E. & McClelland, J.L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests of the model. Psychological Review, 89, 60–94.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Salasoo, A. & Pisoni, D.B. (1985). Interaction of knowledge sources in spoken word identification. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 210–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, A.G. (1981a). Phonemic restoration: Insights from a new methodology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 474–494.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, A.G. (1981b). The role of bottom-up confirmation in the phonemic restoration illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1124–1131.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, A.G. (1986). The role of the lexicon in speech perception. In E.C. Schwab & H.C. Nusbaum (eds.), Pattern recognition by humans and machines: Speech perception, Voll. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, A.G. (1996). Does lexical information influence the perceptual restoration of phonemes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125(1), 28–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P.J. (1991). Contextual constraint and lexical processing. In G.B. Simpson (ed.), Understanding word and sentence. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. (1985). The time course of information activation and utilization in visual word recognition. In D. Besner, T.G. Waller & G.E. MacKinnon (eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice. Vol.5, New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S., Tanenhaus, M.K., Leiman, J.M. & Bienkowski, M. (1982). Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in contexts: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 489–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S., Waters, G.S., Sanders, M. & Langer, P. (1984). Pre- and postlexical loci of contextual effects on word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 12, 315–328.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shubert, R.E. & Eimas, P.D. (1977). Effects of context on the classification of words and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 27–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, G.B. (1984). Lexical ambiguity and its role in model of word recognition. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 316–340.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, G.B. (1994). Context and the processing of ambiguous words. In M.A. Gernsbacher (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. & West, R.E (1981). The effects of sentence context on ongoing word recognition: Test of a two-process theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 658–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D.A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645–659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D.A. & Hakes, D.T. (1976). Effects of prior context upon lexical access during sentence comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 681–689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D., Onifer, W., Prather, P. & Hirshkowitz, M. (1978). Semantic facilitation across sensory modalities in the processing of individual words and sentences. Memory & Cognition, 7, 165–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P. (1988). Accessing lexical ambiguity in different types of sentential context. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 324–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P. (1991). Understanding words in context. In G.B. Simpson (ed.), Understanding word and sentence. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P., Colombo, L. & Job, R. (1987). Accessing lexical ambiguity: Effects of context and dominance. Psychological Research, 49, 161–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi, P. & Zardon, F. (1993). Processing ambiguous words in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 359–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M.K., Dell, G.S. & Carlson, G. (1987). Context effects in lexical processing: A connectionist approach to modularity. In J.L. Garfield (ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M.K. & Donnenwerth-Nolan, S. (1984). Syntactic context and lexical access. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 649–661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M.K., Carlson, G.N. & Seidenberg, M.S. (1985). Do listeners compute linguistic representations? In D.R. Dowty, L. Kartunnen & A.M. Zwicky (eds.), Natural language parsing: Psycholinguistic, theoretical and computational perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M.K., Leiman, J.M. & Seidenberg, M.S. (1979). Evidence for multiple stages in the processing of ambiguous words in syntactic contexts. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 427–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M.K. & Lucas, M.M. (1987). Context effects in lexical processing. Cognition, 25, 213–234.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. (1984). The structure of the initial cohort: Evidence from gating. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 417–427.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. (1990). The relationship between sentential context and sensory input: Comments on Connine’s and Samuel’s chapters. In G.T.M. Altmann (ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1977). The on-line effect of semantic context on syntactic processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 683–692.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1982). Processing utterances in discourse contexts: On-line resolution of anaphors. Journal of Semantics, 1, 297–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1986). The effects of context on the recognition of polymorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 49–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. & Wessels, J. (1983). Quantifying contextual contributions to word recognition processes. Perception and Psychophysics, 34, 409–420.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K. & Wessels, J. (1985). Is gating an on-line task? Evidence from naming-latency data. Perception and Psychophysics, 38, 217–222.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van Petten, C. (1993). A comparison of lexical and sentence-level context effects in event-related potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 485–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Petten, C. & Kutas, M. (1987). Ambiguous words in context: An event-related potential analysis of the time course of meaning activation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 188–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Petten, C. & Kutas, M. (1991). Electrophysiological evidence for the flexibility of lexical processing. In G.B. Simpson (ed.), Understanding word and sentence. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A.C. & Metsala, J.L. (1990). The growth of lexical constraints on spoken word recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 47(3), 267–280.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, R.M. (1970). Perceptual restoration of missing speech sounds. Science, 167, 392–393.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, R.M. (1976). Auditory illusions and perceptual processes. In N.J. Lass (ed.), Contemporary issues in experimental phonetics. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood, P. (1989a). The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken word processing. Cognition, 32, 25–64.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood, P. (1989b). Words and sentences: The effects of sentential semantic context on spoken word processing. Nijmegen, Netherlands: Stichting Studentenpers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood, P. (1994). Access to phonological form representations in language comprehension and production. In C. Clifton, L. Frazier & K. Rayner (eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood, P. & Schriefers, H. (1995). Effects of sensory information and processing time in spoken-word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10(2), 121–136.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Zwitserlood, P. (1998). Spoken Words in Sentence Contexts. In: Language Comprehension: A Biological Perspective. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-97734-3_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-97734-3_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-97736-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-97734-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics