Advertisement

The Speech Code and the Physiology of Language

  • Ignatius G. Mattingly
  • Alvin M. Liberman

Abstract

To the physiologist who would study language in terms of the interests represented at this symposium, the most obvious linguistic processes—the selection of words to convey meaning and the arrangement of words in sentences—must seem far removed from familiar concepts and methods. Surely, he would prefer to study processes that are physiologically more accessible, but are yet linguistic. We believe that the production and perception of speech, in the narrow sense, is one such process; we suggest, therefore, that the physiologist might do well to start there. The questions we would have him ask can be put very simply: How does a speaker convert the phonetic units—the consonants and vowels—to a stream of sound? On hearing that stream, how does a listener recover the phonetic units?

Keywords

Acoustic Signal Speech Perception Deep Structure Vocal Tract Categorical Perception 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson, A. and Lisker, L. (in press). Discrimination along the voicing continuum: cross-language tests. Proc. 6th Int. Cong. Phonetic Sei., Prague, 1967.Google Scholar
  2. Broadbent, D. E. and Gregory, M. (1964), Accuracy of recognition for speech presented to the right and left ears. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 16: 359–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bryden, M. P. (1963). Ear preference in auditory perception. J. Exp. Psychol., 65: 103–105.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Chaney, R. B. and Webster, J. C. (1965). Information in certain multidimensional acoustic signals. Report #1339, U.S. Navy Electronics Laboratory Reports, San Diego.Google Scholar
  5. Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  6. Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York-Evanston-London: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  8. Chomsky, N. and Miller, G. A. (1963). Introduction to the formal analysis of natural languages. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush and E. Galanter (eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology. New York: John Wiley, 2: 269–321.Google Scholar
  9. Coffey, J. L. (1963). The development and evaluation of the Batelle aural reading device. Proc. Int. Cong. Tech. and Blindness I. New York: American Foundation for the Blind, 343–360.Google Scholar
  10. Cooper, F. S. (1950). Research on reading machines for the blind. In P. A. Zahl (ed.), Blindness: modern approaches to the unseen environment. Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 512–543.Google Scholar
  11. Cooper, F. S. (1966). Describing the speech process in motor command terms (abstract). J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 39: 1221 (Text: Status Report on Speech Research SR-5/6, Haskins Laboratories, New York, 1966).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Delattre, P. C, Liberman, A. M. and Cooper, F. S. (1955). Acoustic loci and transitional cues for consonants. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 27: 769–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Freiberger, J. and Murphy, E. F. (1961). Reading machines for the blind. IRE Professional Group on Human Factors in Electronics, HFE-2: 8–19.Google Scholar
  14. Kimura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and perception of verbal stimuli. Canad. J. Psychol., 15: 166–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kimura, D. (1964). Left-right differences in the perception of melodies. Quart. J. Exper. Psychol., 16: 355–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kozhevnikov, V. A. and Chistovich, L. A. (1965). Rech’ Artikuliatsia i vos-priiatie. Moscow-Leningrad. (Trans, as Speech: articulation and perception. Washington: Joint Publications Research Service, 1965).Google Scholar
  17. Lamb, S. (1966). Linguistic structure and the production and decoding of discourse. In E. C. Carterette (ed.), Brain function III speech, language and communication. Berkeley-Los Angeles: U.C.L.A. Press, 173–199.Google Scholar
  18. Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P. and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychol. Rev., 74: 431–461.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Liberman, A. M., Delattre, P. C, Cooper, F. S. and Gerstman, L. J. (1954). The role of consonant-vowel transitions in the perception of the stop and nasal consonants. Psychol. Monogr., 68 (8, Whole, No. 379).Google Scholar
  20. Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S. and Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. J. Exp. Psychol., 54: 358–368.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Kinney, J. S. and Lane, H. (1961). The discrimination of relative onset time of the components of certain speech and nonspeech patterns. J. Exp. Psychol., 61: 379–388.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Lieberman, P. (1967). Intonation, perception, and language. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Lieberman, P. (1968). Primate vocalizations and human linguistic ability. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 44: 1574–1584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lieberman, P., Klatt, D. L. and Wilson, W. A. (in press). Vocal-tract limitations of the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkeys and other non-human primates. Status Report on Speech Research SR-15, Haskins Laboratories, New York.Google Scholar
  25. Lisker, L. and Abramson, A. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word, 20: 384–422.Google Scholar
  26. Mattingly, I. G. (1968). Synthesis by rule of general American English. Supplement to Status Report on Speech Research, Haskins Laboratories, New York.Google Scholar
  27. Mattingly, I. G., Liberman, A. M., Syrdal, A. K. and Halwes, T. (in press). Discrimination of F2 transitions in speech context and in isolation (abstract). J. Acoust. Soc. Amer.Google Scholar
  28. Nye, P. W. (1965). An investigation of audio outputs for a reading machine. Autonomics Division, National Physical Laboratory, Teddington, England.Google Scholar
  29. Nye, P. W. (1968). Research on reading aids for the blind—a dilemma. Med. Biol. Engineering, 6: 43–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ohman, S. E. G. (1966). Coarticulation in VCV utterances: spectrographic measurements. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 39: 151–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Orr, D. B., Friedman, H. L. and Williams, J. C. C. (1965). Trainability of listening comprehension of speeded discourse. J. Educ. Psychol., 56: 148–156.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Shankweiler, D. and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967a). An analysis of perceptual confusions in identification of dichotically presented CVC syllables (abstract). J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 41: 1581 (Text: Status Report on Speech Research SR-10, Haskins Laboratories, New York, 1967).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Shankweiler, D. and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967b). Identification of consonants and vowels presented to left and right ears. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 79:59–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shankweiler, D., Syrdal, A. Halwes, T. and Liberman, A. M. (in progress). Left-right ear effects in the perception of second-formant transitions in and out of speech context.Google Scholar
  35. Stevens, K. N., Liberman, A. M., Ohman, S. E. G. and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (in press). Cross-language study of vowel discrimination. Language and Speech.Google Scholar
  36. Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Cooper, F. S. (1966). High-performance reading machines for the blind: psychological problems, technological problems and status. Proc. St. Dunstan’s Int. Conf. on Sensory Devices for the Blind (London), 317–342.Google Scholar
  37. Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Liberman, A. M. (1963). Psychological considerations in the design of auditory displays for reading machines. Proc. Int. Cong. Techn. and Blindness I. New York: American Foundation for the Blind, 289–304.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1969

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ignatius G. Mattingly
    • 1
  • Alvin M. Liberman
    • 2
  1. 1.Haskins Laboratories and University of ConnecticutUSA
  2. 2.Haskins LaboratoriesUniversity of Connecticut, Yale UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations