Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Research in Criminology ((RESEARCH CRIM.))

Abstract

Perseverance can be assumed to often have an appreciable effect on the thought processes involved in judicial decision making. The phenomenon reflects the everyday experience that “people often do not believe evidence that opposes some theory they hold” (Nisbett & Ross, 1980, p. 169). Perseverance can be described (cf. Ross et al., 1975) as the tendency for information the decision maker initially considered crucial to a decision to retain its influence even when shown to be useless or irrelevant. Obviously, such a tendency runs counter to the basic aim in the judicial process, that of searching for truth (cf. Herrmann, 1971; note that no distinction is made here between material and formal truth). It is often assumed, nevertheless, that a judge’s or jury’s processing of the information relevant to a case is done objectively and that “extra-legal variables” play no appreciable role.

The research described in this contribution was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, N.H., & Barrios, A.A. (1961). Primacy effects in personality impression formation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 346–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandilla, W. (1986). Kontextabhängige Informationsverarbeitung im bundesdeutschen Strafverfahren. Dissertation thesis, University Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, J. (1980). Novelle zur Strafprozeßordnung - Alternativentwurf, Tübingen: Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumberg, A. (1967). The practice of law as confidence game. Law and Society Review, 1, 15–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston: Row, Peterson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, D. (1981). Informationssuche und Informationsbewertung bei Entscheidungen. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, A., & Marcus, M. (1977). The myth of judical supervision in three “inquisitorial” systems: France, Italy and Germany. Yale Law Journal, 87, 240–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, E. (1981). Whodunit? Memory for evidence in text. American Journal of Psychology, 94, 479–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haisch, J. (1977). Die Verarbeitung strafrechtlich relevanter Informationen durch Juristen und Laien in simulierten Gerichtsverfahren. Archiv für Psychologie, 129, 110–119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haisch, J. (1983). Richterliche Urteilsbildung. In W. Seitz (Ed.), Kriminal-und Rechtspsychologie. Ein Handbuch in Schlüsselbegriffen. Munich: Urban and Schwarzenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatvany, N., & Strack, F. (1980). The impact of a discredited key witness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 490–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, R. M., & Anderson, J. K. (1979). On the evaluation of procedural systems in laboratory experiments–a critique of Thibaut and Walker. Law and Human Behavior, 3, 21–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, J. (1971). Die Reform der deutschen Hauptverhandlung nach dem Vorbild des anglo-amerikanischen Strafverfahrens. Bonn: Roehrscheid.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Tour, S. (1978). Determinants of participants and observer statisfaction with adversary and inquisitional modes of adjudication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1531–1545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E.A., & Lissak, R.I. (1985). Apparent impropriety and procedural fairness judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 19–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: strategies and shortcomings of social judgement. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saito, S. (1984). Der japanische Strafprozeß als Kompromiß zwischen anglo-amerikanischem und deutschem Strafprozeßmodell. In M. Irle (Ed.), Sozialwissenschaftliche Entscheidungsforschung. University Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, K. J. (1977). Der Handel mit Gerechtigkeit. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schünemann, B. (1983). Experimentelle Untersuchungen zur Reform der Hauptverhandlung in Strafsachen. In H. J. Kerner, H. Kury, K. Sessar (Eds.), Deutsche Forschungen zur Kriminalitätsentstehung und Kriminalitätskontrolle, 1109–1152. Köln: Heymanns.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schünemann, B. (1985). Kognition, Einstellung und Vorurteil bei der Rechtsfindung. In E.-J. Lampe (Ed.), Beiträge zur Rechtsanthropologie, 68–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S.E., & Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic basis of social information processing. In E. T. Higgins, P. Herman, M. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: the Ontario symposium (Vol. 1 ). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: a psychological analysis. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1978). A theory of procedure. California Law Review, 66, 541–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lersky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weissmann, U. (1982). Die Stellung des Vorsitzenden in der Hauptverhandlung. Die Entwicklung einer Verfahrensordnung zum Wechselverhör mit einer empirischen Studie über den „Passivrichter“. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyer, R. S. (1980). The acquisition and use of social knowledge. Basic postulates and representative research. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 558–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyer, R. S., Srull, T. K. (1981). Category accessibility: some theoretical and empirical issues concerning the processing of social information. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: the Ontario symposium (Vol. 1) ( 1980 ), Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schünemann, B., Bandilla, W. (1989). Perseverance in Courtroom Decisions. In: Wegener, H., Lösel, F., Haisch, J. (eds) Criminal Behavior and the Justice System. Research in Criminology. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-86017-1_12

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-86017-1_12

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-86019-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-86017-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics