Advertisement

Instability and Cognitive Order Formation: Self-Organization Principles, Psychological Experiments, and Psychotherapeutic Interventions

  • Peter Kruse
  • Michael Stadler
  • Boris Pavleković
  • Vladimir Gheorghiu
Part of the Springer Series in Synergetics book series (SSSYN, volume 58)

Abstract

The transfusion of basic principles of self-organization theory to cognitive processes is a necessary and promising task of general and applied psychology. For historical reasons and because of practical evidence self-organization concepts have already gained considerable acceptance in psychotherapy. But the attempt to link the basic theoretical principles with cognitive phenomena and with concrete therapeutic interventions is still in its infancy. The interventions often seem to be more a product of the therapists intuition than a consistent deduction, and in experimental psychology only few examples of phenomena analysed in terms of self-organization theory can be mentioned.

In self-organization theory the dimension of stability and instability of system behavior is of central importance. Systemic instability is a critical agent of order formation. In the phase of instability the system becomes sensitive to minute influences in the sense of little cause having great effect. For psychological processes it can be hypothesized that any cognitive organization or reorganization is guided by phases of internal instability and that the impact of external fluctuations varies with the degree of this instability. As a concrete example, a close connection between suggestibility and cognitive instability can be postulated.

In this report experiments are described to prove these assumptions and some theoretical and practical consequences of the empirical results are discussed. The idea of a synergetic approach to psychotherapy is outlined. The special therapeutic relevance of hypnosis and ritual behavior is briefly explained.

Keywords

Apparent Change Order Formation Multistable Pattern Gestalt Theory Perceptual Alternative 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. BERGUM, J.E. & BERGUM, B.O.1980. Reliability of reversal rates as a measure of perceptual stability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 1038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. BRUNNER, E.J. 1988. Pioniere systemischen Denkens. In: Reiter, L., Brunner, E.J. & Reiter-Theil, S. (Eds.): Von der Familientherapie zur systemischen Perspektive. Berlin: Springer, 273–284.Google Scholar
  3. COFFIN, T.E. 1941. Some conditions of suggestion and suggestibility: A study of certain attitudinal and situational factors influencing the process of suggestion. Psychological Monographs, 4, 53.Google Scholar
  4. CRAWFORD, H.J. 1989. Cognitive and physiological flexibility: multiple pathways to hypnotic responsiveness. In: Gheorghiu, V.A., Netter, P., Eysenck, HJ. & Rosenthal, R. (Eds.): Suggestion and Suggestibility. Berlin: Springer, 155–167.Google Scholar
  5. Depascalis, W. 1990. Sensory suggestibility and self-organization. 5th European Congress of Hypnosis in Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine. University of Constance, FRG.Google Scholar
  6. FEINGOLD, E. 1982. Untersuchungen zur sensorischen Suggestibilitt sowie zum Zusammenhang zwischen sensorischer Suggestibility und der Placebo-Ansprechbarkeit im Schmerzbereich. Mainz: Doctoral Dissertation.Google Scholar
  7. GHEORGHIU, V.A. & KRUSE, P. 1991. The psychology of suggestion: An integrative perspective. In: Schumaker, J.F. (Ed.): Human Suggestibility. Advances in Theory, Research, and Application. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  8. GHEORGHIU, V.A. & REYHER, J. 1982. The effect of different types of influence of an “indirect-direct” form of a scale of sensory suggestibility. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 24, 191–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. GRÄSER, H. 1977. Spontane Reversionsprozesse in der Figuralwahrnehmung. Trier: Doctoral Dissertation.Google Scholar
  10. GRAWE, K. 1987. Psychotherapie als Entwicklungsstimulation von Schemata. Ein Prozeß mit nicht vorhersehbarem Ausgang. In: Caspar, F. (Ed.) Problemanalyse in der Psychotherapie. Tübingen: DGVT, 72–87.Google Scholar
  11. HAKEN, H. 1977. Synergetics. An Introduction. Berlin: Springer.zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  12. HAKEN, H. 1979. Pattern formation and pattern recognition — An attempt at a synthesis. In: Haken, H. (Ed): Pattern Formation by Dynamic Systems and Pattern Recognition. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. HAKEN, H. 1991. Synergetic Computers and Cognition. Berlin: Springer.zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  14. HAKEN, H. & STADLER, M. (Eds.) 1990. Synergetics of Cognition. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  15. HEATH, H.A. & ORBACH, J. 1963. Reversibility of the Necker cube: IV. Responses of elderly people. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 17, 625–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. KLINTMAN, H. 1984. Original thinking and ambiguous figure reversal rates. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 129–131.Google Scholar
  17. KÖHLER, W. 1938a. Some gestalt problems. In: Ellis, W.D. (Ed.): A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  18. KÖHLER, W. 1938b. The Place of Value in a World of Facts. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
  19. KÖHLER, W. 1940. Dynamics in Psychology. New York: LiverightGoogle Scholar
  20. KÖHLER, W. 1955. Direction of processes in living systems. Scientific Monthly, 80, 29–32.ADSGoogle Scholar
  21. KRUSE, P. 1986. Wie unabhängig ist das Wahrnehmungsobjekt vom Prozeß der Identifikation. Gestalt Theory, 8, 141–143.Google Scholar
  22. KRUSE, P. 1988. Stabilität, Instabilität, MultiStabilität. Selbstorganisation und Selbstreferentialität in kognitiven Systemen. Delfin, 11, 35–57.Google Scholar
  23. KRUSE, P. 1989. Some suggestions about suggestion and hypnosis. In: Gheorghiu, V.A., Netter, P., Eysenck, H.J. & Rosenthal, R. (Eds.): Suggestion and Suggestibility. Berlin: Springer, 91–98.Google Scholar
  24. KRUSE, P. & GHEORGHIU, V.A. 1990. Self-organization theory and radical constructivism: A new concept for understanding hypnosis, suggestion, and suggestibility. 5th European Congress of Hypnosis in Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine. University of Constance, FRG.Google Scholar
  25. KRUSE, P. & STADLER, M. 1990. Stability and instability in cognitive systems: multi-stability, suggestion and psychosomatic interaction. In: Haken, H. & Stadler, M. (Eds.): Synergetics of Cognition. Berlin: Springer, 201–215.Google Scholar
  26. KRUSE, P., STADLER, M. & WEHNER, T. 1986. Direction and frequency specific processing in the perception of long-range apparent movement. Vision Research, 26, 327–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. KRUSE, P., ROTH, G. & STADLER, M. 1988. Self-organization and Köhler’s psychophysical field theory. German Journal of Psychology, 12, 334–336.Google Scholar
  28. KRUSE, P., STADLER, M. & STRÜBER, D. 1991. Psychological modification and synergetic modelling of perceptual oscillations. In: Haken, H. & Koepchen, H.P. (Eds.): Synergetics of Rhythm in Biological Systems. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  29. LEWIN, K. 1936. Principles of Topological Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. MAHONEY, MJ. 1974. Cognition and Behavior Modification. Cambridge, Mass.: Ballinger.Google Scholar
  31. MILLER, G.A., GALANTER, E. & PRIBRAM, K.H. 1960. Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York: Henry Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. NEBELYTSYN, V.D. 1964. An investigation of the connection between sensitivity and strength of the nervous system. In: Gray, J.A. (Ed): Pavlov’s Typology. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 402–445.Google Scholar
  33. PAVLOV, LP. 1955. Selected Works. Moscow: Foreign Language Publishing House.Google Scholar
  34. PRIGOGINE, I. 1980. From Being to Becoming. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  35. RAMACHANDRAN, V.S. & ANSTIS, S.M. 1985. Perceptual organization in multistable apparent motion. Perception, 14, 135–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. REITER, L., BRUNNER, E.J. & REITER-THEDL, S. (Eds.): 1988. Von der Familientherapie zur systemischen Perspektive. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  37. SCHNEIDER, H. 1983. Auf dem Wege zu einem neuen Verständnis des psychotherapeutischen Prozesses. Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
  38. SHERIF, M. 1935. A study of some social factors in perception. Archives of Psychology, 187, 60.Google Scholar
  39. SIMON, F.B. 1988. Unterschiede die Unterschiede machen. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  40. STADLER, M. & KRUSE, P. 1990a. Cognitive systems as self-organizing systems. In: Krohn, W. & Küppers, G. (Eds.): Self-organization. Portrait of a Scientific Revolution. Dordrecht: Kluwer, 181–193.Google Scholar
  41. STADLER, M. & KRUSE, P. 1990b. Theory of Gestalt and self-organization. In: Heylighen, F., Rosseel, E. & Demeyere, F. (Eds.): Self-Steering and Cognition in Complex Systems. Towards a New Cybernetics. New York: Gordon and Breach, 142–159.Google Scholar
  42. STADLER, M. & KRUSE, P. 1990c. The self-organization perspective in cognition research: historical remarks and new experimental approaches. In: Haken, H. & Stadler, M. (Eds.): Synergetics of Cognition. Berlin: Springer, 32–52.Google Scholar
  43. STADLER, M., RICHTER, P. H., PFAFF, S. & KRUSE, P. 1991. Attractors and perceptual field dynamics of homogeneous stimulus areas. Psychological Research, 53.Google Scholar
  44. STADLER, M., VOGT, S. & KRUSE, P. 1991. Synchronization of rhythm in motor actions. In: Haken, H. & Koepchen, H.P. (Eds.): Synergetics of Rhythm in Biological Systems. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  45. VOGT, S., STADLER, M. & KRUSE, P. 1988. Self-organization aspects in the temporal formation of movement Gestalts. Human Movement Science, 7, 365–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. VON DER MALSBURG, C. 1984. Algorithms, brain, and organization. In: Demongeot, J., Goles, E. & Tchuente, M. (Eds.): Colloque sur le compartement dynamique des réseaux d’automates. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  47. WIENER, N. 1948. Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine. New York: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  48. WUNDT, W. 1898. Zur Theorie der räumlichen Gesichtsempfindungen. Philosophische Studien, 14, 1–118.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Peter Kruse
  • Michael Stadler
  • Boris Pavleković
  • Vladimir Gheorghiu

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations