Advertisement

Individual Differences in Experience-Producing Tendencies

  • Bruce Henderson

Abstract

More than a decade ago, presenters at a conference on imagination, play and curiosity (e.g., Keller, 1987; Wohlwill, 1987) suggested that more attention needed to be paid to individual differences in curiosity and exploration. The presence of a section on individual differences in the present volume indicates that those earlier suggestions did not fall on deaf ears. In this chapter, I argue that individual differences in curiosity and exploration represent an important subcategory of behaviors that I will call experience-producing tendencies. I then review some preliminary research on the relation between individual differences in the tendency to produce experiences through exploration and individual differences in measured intelligence and achievement. My interpretation of the role of exploration in intellectual development is tentative and speculative in nature, but it is consistent with some recent views of how children create their own environments and thus influence their own intellectual abilities (e.g., Anastasi, 1985; Plomin, 1986; Scarr & McCartney, 1983).

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anastasi, A. (1985). Reciprocal relations between cognitive and affective development: With implications for sex differences. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Psychology and gender (Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 32, pp. 1–35). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  2. Berg, C., & Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Response to novelty: Continuity versus discontinuity in the developmental course of intelligence. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 19, pp. 1–47). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bornstein, M. H., & Krasnegor, N. A. (Eds.). (1989). Stability and continuity in mental development: Behavioral and biological perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Caruso, D. A. (1989). Attachment and exploration in infancy: Research and applied issues. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 4, 117–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Coffman, W. (1985). Review of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. In J. Mitchell, Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (Vol. 1, pp. 771–773). Lincoln, NE: The University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  6. Coie, J. D. (1974). An evaluation of the cross-sectional stability of children’s curiosity. Journal of Personality, 42, 93–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dollinger, S. J., & Seiters, J. A. (1988). Intrinsic motivation among clinic-referred children. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 449–451.Google Scholar
  8. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Endsley, R. C., Hutcherson, M. A., Garner, A. P., & Martin, M. J. (1979). Interrelationships among selected maternal behaviors, authoritarianism, and preschool children’s verbal and nonverbal curiosity. Child Development, 50, 331–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Harter, S., & Zigler, E. (1974). The assessment of effectance motivation in normal and retarded children. Developmental Psychology, 10, 169–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hayes, K. J. (1962). Genes, drives, and intellect. Psychological Reports, 10, 299–342.Google Scholar
  12. Henderson, B. B. (1984a). Parents and exploration: The effect of context on individual differences in exploratory behavior. Child Development, 55, 1237–1245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Henderson, B. B. (1984b). Social support and exploration. Child Development, 55, 1246–1251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Henderson, B. B. (1988). Individual differences in exploration: A replication and extension. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 149, 555–557.Google Scholar
  15. Henderson, B. B., Gold, S. R., & McCord, M. T. (1982). Daydreaming and curiosity in gifted and average children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 18, 576–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Henderson, B. B., & Moore, S. G. (1979). Measuring exploratory behavior in young children: A factor analytic study. Developmental Psychology, 15, 113–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Henderson, B. B., & Wilson, S. (1991). Intelligence and curiosity in preschool children. Journal of School Psychology, 29, 167–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Inagaki, K. (1978). Effects of object curiosity on exploratory learning in young children. Psychological Reports, 42, 899–908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kagan, J., Sontag, L. W., Baker, C. T., & Nelson, V. L. (1958). Personality and IQ change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 261–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (1983). Interpretive manual for the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  21. Keller, J. A. (1987). Motivational aspects of exploratory behavior. In D. Gorlitz, & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Curiosity, imagination, and play (pp. 24–42). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Kreitler, S., Zigler, E., & Kreitler, H. (1975). The nature of curiosity in children. Journal of School Psychology, 13, 185–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Langevin, R. (1971). Is curiosity a unitary construct? Canadian Journal of Psychology, 25, 360–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Martin, C. E., & Henderson, B. B. (1989). Adult support and the exploratory behavior of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 67–68.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Maw, W. H., & Maw, E. W. (1975). Note on curiosity and intelligence of school children. Psychological Reports, 36, 782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Maw, W. H., & Maw, E. W. (1977). Nature and assessment of curiosity. In P. McReynolds (Ed.), Advances in psychological assessment (Vol. 4, pp. 526–571). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  27. Penney, R. K., & McCann, B. (1964). The Children’s Reactive Curiosity Scale. Psychological Reports, 15, 323–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pielstick, N. L., & Woodruff, A. B. (1964). Exploratory behavior and curiosity in two age and ability groups of children. Psychological Reports, 14, 831–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Plomin, R. (1986). Development, genetics, and psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  30. Scarr, S. (1981). Testing for children: Assessment and the many determinants of intellectual competence. American Psychologist, 36, 1159–1166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Sternberg, R. J. (1984). The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children: An information processing analysis and critique. Journal of Special Education, 18, 267–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Vandenberg, B. R. (1985). The effects of retardation on exploration. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 397–409.Google Scholar
  34. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Wohlwill, J. F. (1987). Varieties of exploratory activity in early childhood. In D. Gorlitz, & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Curiosity, imagination, and play (pp. 59–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  • Bruce Henderson

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations