Advertisement

Synchronization of Rhythm in Motor Actions

  • M. Stadler
  • S. Vogt
  • P. Kruse
Conference paper
Part of the Springer Series in Synergetics book series (SSSYN, volume 55)

Abstract

Motor rhythms studied on the level of behavioral phenomenology are surveyed in this paper and alternative views to the concept of motor programming are presented. First, v. Hoist’s concept of coupled oscillators and the Fourier coding model are discussed. The experimental data presented do not confirm frequency-specific processing if dissociated from the phase angle information. Then physical and cognitive principles of self-organization in the motor domain are investigated. Following the results of experimental studies the principle of exploitation of kinetic energy seems to be an organizing factor in motor actions. Furthermore the results of serial reproduction experiments show that the cyclical movement rhythms converge to the same eigen-structure of simple harmonic relations. Finally it is concluded that there is a transition in the development of goal-directed action from goal-adjustment to goal-attraction.

Keywords

Couple Oscillator Frequency Selective Channel Motor Rhythm Motor Organization Frequency Composition 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Reference

  1. [1]
    K. Koffka: Principles of Gestalt Psychology, (London, Routledge & Kegan Paul 1935).Google Scholar
  2. At the same time Lewin used the terms ’psychological field’ or ’life space’. K. Lewin: Dynamic Theory of Personality, (McGraw-Hill, New York 1935)Google Scholar
  3. [2]
    J.A.S. Kelso, G. Schöner: Toward a physical (synergetic) theory of biological coordination, in Lasers and Synergetics, ed. by R. Graham, (Springer, Berlin 1987), pp. 224–237Google Scholar
  4. [3]
    E. v. Holst: Naturwissenschaften 25, 625–631; 641–647 (1937)ADSCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. E. v. Holst: Erg. Physiol. 42, 228–306 (1939)Google Scholar
  6. see also C.R. Gallistel: The Organization of Action: a New Synthesis, (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J. 1980)Google Scholar
  7. [4]
    N. Bernstein: The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movement (Pergamon Press, Oxford 1967)Google Scholar
  8. see also K.H. Pribram: Languages of the Brain, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J. 1971Google Scholar
  9. H.T.A. Whiting: Human Motor Actions: Bernstein Reassessed, (North-Holland, Amsterdam 1984)Google Scholar
  10. [5]
    W. Köhler: Die physischen Gestalten in Ruhe und im stationären Zustand, (Vieweg, Braunschweig 1920)Google Scholar
  11. D. Katz: Gestaltpsychologie (Schwabe, Stuttgart 1969),4th ed.Google Scholar
  12. W. Metzger: Gestalt-Psychologie, ed by M. Stadler, H. Crabus, (Kramer, Frankfurt/M. 1986)Google Scholar
  13. see also S. Vogt, M. Stadler, P. Kruse: Human Movement Sci. 7, 365–406 (1988)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. [6]
    H. Haken: Synergetics, an Introduction, (Springer, Berlin 1977)MATHGoogle Scholar
  15. H. Haken: Advanced Synergetics, (Springer, Berlin 1983)MATHGoogle Scholar
  16. [7]
    H. Haken, J.A.S. Kelso, H. Bunz: Biol Cybernetics 51, 347–356 (1985)MathSciNetMATHCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. J.A.S. Kelso, G. Schöner: Human Movement Sci. 7, 27–46 (1988)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. J.J. Jeka, J.A.S. Kelso: The dynamic pattern approach to coordinated behavior: a tutorial review, in Perspectives on the Coordination of Movement, ed. by S.A. Wallace, (North-Holland, Amsterdam 1989) see Kelso in this volumeGoogle Scholar
  19. E. v. Hoist (1939), see note 3Google Scholar
  20. [9]
    T. Wehner, M. Stadler: Z. Psychol. 190,183–201 (1982)Google Scholar
  21. see note 3Google Scholar
  22. see note 4Google Scholar
  23. [12]
    see notes 3 and 4Google Scholar
  24. [13]
    K.H. Pribram, A. Sharafat, G.J. Beekmann: Frequency encoding in motor systems, in Human Motor Actions: Bernstein Reassessed, ed. by H.T.A. Whiting (North-Holland, Amsterdam 1984), pp. 121–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. [14]
    G. Johansson: Perception and Psychophysics 14, 202–211 (1973)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. [15]
    P. Kruse, M. Stadler, S. Vogt, T. Wehner: Gestalt Theory 5, 83–113 (1983)Google Scholar
  27. P. Kruse, M. Stadler, T: Wehner: Vision Res. 26,327–335 (1986)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. W. Metzger (1986), see note 5Google Scholar
  29. K.H. Pribram, Towards a holonomic theory of perception, in Gestalttheorie in der modernen Psychologie, ed. by S. Ertel, L. Kemmler, M. Stadler (Steinkopff, Darmstadt 1975), pp. 161–184Google Scholar
  30. [17]
    H. Haken: Information and Self-organization, (Springer, Berlin 1988), pp. 151f.MATHGoogle Scholar
  31. S. Vogt: Einige gestaltpsychologische Aspekte der zeitlichen Organisation zyklischer Bewegungsabläufe (Doct. Thesis, Bremen 1988), ch. 4Google Scholar
  32. [19]
    R.G. Marteniuk, S.K.E. Romanow: Human movement organization and learning as revealed by variability of movement, use of kinematic information, and Fourier analysis, in Memory and Control of Action, ed. by RA. Magill, (North-Holland, Amsterdam 1983)Google Scholar
  33. [20]
    M.L. Stanley, I.M. Franks: Learning to organize the frequency components of a perceptual motor skill (in press)Google Scholar
  34. see also H.T.A. Whiting, S. Vogt, B. Vereijken: Human skill and motor control: some aspects of the motor control – motor learning relation, in Approaches to the Study of Motor Control and Learning, ed. by. J.J. Summers (North-Holland, Amsterdam 1991, in press)Google Scholar
  35. W. Metzger, see note 5Google Scholar
  36. R.N. Shepard: Psychological Review 91,417–447 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. K.H. Pribram: Prolegomenon for a holonomic brain theory, in Synergetics of Cognition, ed. by H. Haken, M. Stadler, (Springer, Berlin 1990), addendum p. 178Google Scholar
  38. N. Bernstein, in H.T.A. Whiting (1984), p. 101, see note 4Google Scholar
  39. [24]
    H.T.A. Whiting, B. den Brinker: Image of the act, in Theory and Research in Learning Disabilities, ed. by J.P. Das, R.F. Mulcahy, A.E. Wall (Plenum, New York 1982)Google Scholar
  40. see e.g. Whiting & den Brinker’s integration of Bernstein’s view and Pribram’s (1971), note 4, concept of an ‘image of achievement’, and our discussion of the Fourier coding model in section 3Google Scholar
  41. see discussion in H.T.A. Whiting, S. Vogt, B. Vereijken, note 20Google Scholar
  42. H. Haken (1988), see note 17Google Scholar
  43. [27]
    M.T. Turvey: Preliminaries of a theory of action with reference to vision, in Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing, ed. by R. Shaw, J. Bransford, (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J. 1977)Google Scholar
  44. [28]
    J.A.S. Kelso, J.P. Scholz: Cooperative phenomena in biological motion, in Complex Systems – Operational Approaches, ed. by H. Haken, (Springer, Berlin 1985), pp. 124–149Google Scholar
  45. S. Vogt, M. Stadler, P. Kruse (1988), see note 5Google Scholar
  46. W. Metzger, p. 185, see note 5, English translation by the authorsGoogle Scholar
  47. [31]
    H. Heuer, R.A. Schmidt: J. Exp. Psychol.: Human Perception and Performance 14,241–252 (1988)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. [32]
    P. Viviani: Do units of motor action really exist?, in Generation and Modulation of Action Patterns, ed by H. Heuer, C. Fromm, (Springer, Berlin 1986)Google Scholar
  49. S. Vogt, M. Stadler, P. Kruse (1988), see note 5Google Scholar
  50. The experiments were performed by Z. FrankoGoogle Scholar
  51. M. Stadler, P. Richter, S. Pfaff, P. Kruse: Psychological Research (in press)Google Scholar
  52. The experiments were performed by B. PavlekovicGoogle Scholar
  53. E. v. Hoist (1939), see note 3Google Scholar
  54. [38]
    M. Stadler, P. Kruse: The selforganization perspective in cognition research: Historical remarks and new experimental approaches, in Synergetics of Cognition, ed. by H. Haken, M. Stadler (Springer, Berlin 1990), pp. 32–52Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • M. Stadler
    • 1
  • S. Vogt
    • 2
  • P. Kruse
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of BremenBremen 33Germany
  2. 2.Max Planck Institute for Psychological ResearchMünchen 40Germany

Personalised recommendations