Ultrasound and Infrasound

  • J. D. Pye
  • W. R. LangbauerJr.

Abstract

Ultrasound and infrasound differ from “ordinary” sounds in three distinct ways that influence all the considerations of this chapter. The first and most obvious characteristic of these sound types is that, by definition, they are “extreme” frequencies that fall outside the normal response curve for the human ear (see Figure 1) and are therefore inaudible. Ultrasound, which includes biologically significant sounds ranging from 15 kHz or so up to 200 kHz, is too high in frequency. Infrasound, effectively extending downwards from about 20 to 0.1 Hz or less, is too low in frequency. In both cases, therefore, it is necessary to use special instruments merely to detect the signals, which only increases the fascination of studying them. When the appropriate technology is applied, it becomes possible to observe phenomena that may be quite common among nonhuman species, but have previously been unknown.

Keywords

Attenuation Rubber Radar Nylon Gravel 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andersen BB, Miller LA (1977) A portable ultrasonic detection system for recording bat cries in the field. J Mammal 58:226–229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arabadzhi VL (1990) Migration of birds and infrasound. Biofizika 35:361–362Google Scholar
  3. Ayler D (1971) Noise reduction by vegetation and pound. J Acoust Soc Am 51:197–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bazley EN (1976) Sound absorption in air at frequencies up to 100 kHz. Acoustics report AC74. National Physical Laboratories, Teddington, UKGoogle Scholar
  5. Bowman JJ, Senior TBA, Uslenghi PLE (eds) (1987) Electromagnetic and acoustic scattering by simple shapes. Radiation Laboratory, Univ Michigan, Ann ArborGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown SG (1954) Dispersal in blue and fin whales. Discovery Rep 26:355–384Google Scholar
  7. Busnel RG, Fish JF (eds) (1980) Animal sonar systems. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Coles RB, Guppy A, Anderson ME, Schlegel P (1989) Frequency sensitivity and directional hearing in the gleaning bat,Plecotus auritus (Linnaeus 1758). J Comp Physiol A 165:269–280PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dneprovskaya LA, Iofe VK, Levitas FI (1963) On the attenuation of sound as it propagates through the atmosphere. Sov Phys Acoust 8:235–239Google Scholar
  10. Evans JB, Bass HE (1972) Atmospheric absorption of sound as a function of frequency and relative humidity. Rep no WR 72–2, Wyle Laboratories, Huntsville, AlabamaGoogle Scholar
  11. Eyring CF (1946) Jungle acoustics. J Acoust Soc Amer 18:257–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fenton MB (1985) Communication in the chiroptera. Indiana University Press, BloomingtonGoogle Scholar
  13. Fletcher NH (1992) Acoustic systems in biology. Oxford University Press New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Fletcher NH, Thwaites S (1979) Physical models for the analysis of acoustical systems in biology. Q Rev Biophys 12:25–65PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Galton F (1883) Inquiries into human faculty and its development. Dent, London and Dutton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. George JC, Clark C, Carroll GM, Ellison WT (1989) Observations on the ice-breaking and ice-navigation behavior of migrating bowhead whales (Balaena mysticetus). Arctic 42:24–30Google Scholar
  17. Griffin DR (1958) Listening in the dark. Yale Univ Press, New Haven; republ (1974) Dover Publications, NY; (1986) Comstock/Cornell Univ Press, Ithaca New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Griffin DR (1971) The importance of atmospheric attenuation for the echolocation of bats (Chiroptera). Anim Behav 19:55–61PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hartley DJ, Suthers RA (1988) Directional emission and time precision as a function of target angle in the echolocating batCarolliaperspicillata. In: Nachtigall PE, Moore PWB (eds) Animal sonar: processes and performance. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Hartridge H (1920) The avoidance of objects by bats in their flight. J Physiol 54:54–57PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Heffner SR, Heffher EH (1982) Hearing in the elephant(Elephas maximus): absolute sensitivity, frequency discrimination, and sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol 96:926–944PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ingard U (1953) A review of the influence of meteorological conditions on sound propagation. J Acoust Soc Am 25:405–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jackson RR, Wilcox RS (1990) Aggressive mimicry, prey-specific predatory behaviour and predator—prey interactions ofProtia fimbriatta andEuryttus sp. jumping spiders from Queensland, Australia. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:111–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kreithen ML (1980) Detection of sound and vibration by birds. In: Proc 2nd Conference on Abnormal animal behavior. prior to earthquakes. Open file Rep 80–453, US Geol Surv, Menlo Park, CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
  25. Kreithen ML (1983) Strategies of bird orientation: a tribute to WT Keeton. In: Aspey WP, Lustick SI (eds) Behavioral energetics: vertebrate costs of survival. Ohio State University Press, Columbus, p 3Google Scholar
  26. Kreithen ML, Keaton WT (1974) Detection of changes in atmospheric pressure by the homing pigeon,Columba livia. J Comp Physiol A 89:73–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kreithen ML, Quine DB (1979) Infrasound detection by the homing pigeon: a behavioral audiogram. J Comp Physiol A 129:1–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Langbauer WR Jr, Charif R, Osborn F (in press) Transmission of low-frequency sound in three African environments and its relevance to long distance elephant communication. Behav Ecol Sociobiol (in press)Google Scholar
  29. Langbauer WL Jr, Payne K, Charif R, Rapaport L, Osborn F (1991) African elephants respond to distant playback of low-frequency calls. J Exp Biol 157:34–46Google Scholar
  30. Langbauer WL Jr, Powell SP, Martin R (in review) A radiotelemetry system for monitoring location and vocalizations of elephants. J Wildl ManageGoogle Scholar
  31. Lawrence BD, Simmons JA (1982) Measurements of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. J Acoust Soc Am 71:585–590PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lewis B (1983) Directional cues for auditory localization. In: Lewis B (ed) Bioacoustics: a comparative approach. Academic Press, London, NY, p 233–257Google Scholar
  33. Lubbock J (1879) Observations on the habits of ants, bees and wasps: VI Ants. J Linn Soc Lond Zool 14:607–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lutz FE (1924) Insect sounds. Bull Am Mus Nat Hist 50:333–372Google Scholar
  35. Maries K, Pye JD, Coppen D (1989) A microprocessor-controlled “memory” bat detector. In: Hanak V, Horacek I, Gaisler J (eds) European bat research 1987. Charles University Press, Prague, p 287–290Google Scholar
  36. Marten K, Marier P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization I: temperate habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:271–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Marten K, Quine D, Marier P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization II: tropical forest habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:291–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Maxim HS (1912) A new system for preventing collisions at sea. Cassel, London, NY (also see Sci Amer, July 27, 1912)Google Scholar
  39. Michelsen A (1978) Sound reception in different environments. In: Ali BA (ed) Perspectives in sensory ecology. Plenum, New York, p 345–373Google Scholar
  40. Montgomery HC (1932) Do our ears grow old? Bell Lab Rec 10:311–313Google Scholar
  41. Moss C, Poole JH (1983) Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In: Hinde R (ed) Primate social relationships: an integrated approach. Blackwell Scientific Publ Oxford, p 315–325Google Scholar
  42. Moss R, Lockie I (1979) Infrasonic components in the song of the capercaillie(Tetrao urogallus). Ibis 121:94–97Google Scholar
  43. Nachtigall PE, Moore PWB (eds)(1988) Animal sonar: processes and performance. Plenum Press, NYGoogle Scholar
  44. Neubauer WG (1986) Acoustic reflection from surfaces and shapes. Nav Res Lab, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  45. Packard A, Karlen HE, Sand O (1990) Low frequency hearing in cephalopods. J Comp Physiol A 166:501–505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Patterson B, Hamilton GR (1964) Repetitive 20 cycles per second biological hydroacoustic signal at Bermuda. In: Tavolga WN (ed) Marine bioacoustics: symposium at Lerner Marine Lab, 1963. Macmillan, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  47. Payne K, Langbauer WR Jr, Thomas E (1986) Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:297–301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Payne RS, Webb D (1971) Orientation by means of long-range acoustic signalling in baleen whales. In: Adler HE (ed) Orientation: sensory basis. Ann NY Acad Sci 188:110–141Google Scholar
  49. Pierce GW (1948) The songs of insects. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  50. Pierce GW, Griffin DR (1938) Experimental determination of supersonic notes emitted by bats. J Mammal 19:454–455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pollak GD (1993) Some comments on the proposed perception of phase and nanosecond time disparities by echolocating bats. J Comp Physiol A 172:523–531PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Poole JH, Payne K, Langbauer WR Jr, Moss C (1988) The social context of some very low-frequency calls of African elephants. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:385–392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pye JD (1971) Bats and fog. Nature 229:572–574PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Pye JD (1979) Why ultrasound? Endeavour 3:57–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Pye JD (1980) Echolocation signals and echoes in air. In: Busnel RG, Fish JF (eds) Animal sonar systems. Plenum Press, NY and London, p 309–353Google Scholar
  56. Pye JD (1983) Techniques for studying ultrasound. In: Lewis B (ed) Bioacoustics: a comparative approach. Academic Press, London, NY, p 39–65Google Scholar
  57. Pye JD (1988) Noseleaves and bat pulses. In: Nachtigall PE, Moore PWB (eds) Animal sonar: processes and performance. Plenum Press, NY and London, p 791–796Google Scholar
  58. Pye JD (1992) Equipment and techniques for the study of ultrasound in air. Bioacoustics 4:77–88Google Scholar
  59. Pye JD (1993) Is fidelity futile? The “true” signal is illusory, especially with ultrasound. Bioacoustics 4: 271–286Google Scholar
  60. Pye JD, Mutere FA (1985–6) Recording bat sounds by new techniques. Myotis 23/24:245–248Google Scholar
  61. Pye JD, Pye A (1988) Echolocation sounds and hearing in the fruit bat,Rousettus. In: Stephens SDG, Prasansuk S (eds) Measurement in hearing and balance, Karger, Basel Switzerland, p 1Google Scholar
  62. Rossing TD (1980) Physics and psychophysics of high-fidelity sound, part III. Physics Teach 18:426CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Rossing TD (1983) The science of sound. Addison-Wesley, Reading, p 365Google Scholar
  64. Rydell J, Arlettaz R (1994) Low frequency echolocation enables the batTadarida teniotis to feed on tympanate insects. Proc Roy Soc B 257:175–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sales GD, Pye JD (1974) Ultrasonic communication by animals. Chapman and Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar
  66. Sand O, Karlsen HE (1986) Detection of infrasound by the Atlantic cod. J Exp Biol 125:197–204PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Schermuly L, Klinke R (1990) Infrasound sensitive neurones in the pigeon cochlear ganglion. J Comp Physiol A 166:355–363PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Schevill WE, Watkins WA, Backus RH (1964) Underwater sounds of cetaceans. In: Tavolga WN (ed) Marine bioacoustics. Pergamon Press, New York, p 147Google Scholar
  69. Schnitzler HU, Grinnell AD (1977) Directional sensitivity of echolocation in the horseshoe bat,Rhinolophusferrumequinum, I. Directionality of sound emission. J Comp Physiol A 116:51–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Silver SC, Halls JAT (1980) Recording the sounds of hydropsychid larvae — a cautionary tale. J Comp Physiol A 140:159–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Simmons JA (1993) Evidence for perception of fine echo delay and phase by the FM bat,Eptesicus fuscus. J Comp Physiol A 172:533–47PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Surlikke A, Miller LA (1985) The influence of arctiid moth clicks on bat echolocation; jamming or warning? J Comp Physiol A 156:831–843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Theurich M, Langner G, Scheich H (1984) Infrasound responses in the midbrain of the Guinea fowl. Neurosci Lett 49:81–86PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Waser PM, Waser MS (1977) Experimental studies of primate vocalization: specializations for long-distance propagation. Z Tierpsychol 43:239–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Wiley RH, Richards DG (1978) Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 3:69–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Wiley RH, Richards DG (1982) Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission and signal detection. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds. Vol 1. Academic Press, New York, p 131Google Scholar
  77. Wollaston WH (1820) On sounds inaudible by certain ears. Philos Trans R Soc Lond 110:306–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Yager DD, Hoy RR (1989) The cyclopean ear: a new sense for the praying mantis. Science 231:649–772Google Scholar
  79. Zbinden K (1985–6) Echolocation pulse design in bats and dolphins. Myotis 23–24:195–200Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. D. Pye
  • W. R. LangbauerJr.

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations