Advertisement

Sound Playback Studies

  • S. L. Hopp
  • E. S. Morton

Abstract

In the early 1890s, before Edison publicly released his gramophone, R.L. Garner, a researcher at the newly established National Zoological Park in Washington, D.C., had begun recording animal vocalizations. As part of his effort to decipher “the speech of monkeys”, Garner conducted some of the first sound playback experiments. He reasoned that by replaying the vocalizations to animals and watching their reactions, he could learn the functions of the sounds. In one experiment he replayed the sound of a female rhesus monkey to her mate, who had been separated from visual contact. Garner recounts, “He gave evident signs of recognizing the sounds, and at once began a search for the mysterious monkey doing the talking. His perplexity at this strange affair cannot well be described. The familiar voice of his mate would induce him to approach, but that squeaking, chattering horn was a feature which he could not comprehend. He traced the sounds, however, to the source from which they came, and failing to find his mate, thrust his arm into the horn quite up to his shoulder, then withdrew it, and peeped into it again and again” (Garner 1892, p. 6). Although his methods were simple by today’s standards, and his interpretation of the response not exactly conclusive, Garner nevertheless anticipated what has become one of the most important techniques for studying animal communication, the sound playback experiment.

Keywords

Alarm Call Song Type Vervet Monkey Acoustic Communication Sound Transmission 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adhikerana AS, Slater PJB (1993) Singing interactions in coal tits, Parus ater: an experimental approach. Anim Behav 46:1205–1211Google Scholar
  2. Alatalo RV, Glynn C, Lundberg A (1990) Singing rate and female attraction in the pied flycatcher: an experiment. Anim Behav 39:601–603Google Scholar
  3. Arvidsson BL, Neergaard R (1991) Mate choice in the willow warbler-a field experiment. Behaviour Ecol Sociobiol 29:2251–229Google Scholar
  4. Bakeman R, Gottman JM (1986) Observing interaction. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  5. Baptista LF, Gaunt SLL (1994) Advances in studies of avian sound communication. Condor 96:817–830Google Scholar
  6. Baptista LF, Petrinovich L (1986) Song development in the white-crowned sparrow: social factors and sex differences. Anim Behav 34:1359–1371Google Scholar
  7. Becker PH (1982) The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds. Academic Press, New York, p 213Google Scholar
  8. Beecher MD, Stoddard PK (1990) The role of bird song and calls in individual recognition: contrasting field and laboratory perspectives. In: Stebbins WC, Berkley MA (eds) Comparative perception, vol II. Wiley, New York, p 375Google Scholar
  9. Bilger RC, Hirsh IJ (1956) Masking of tones by bands of noise. J Acoust Soc Am 28:623–630Google Scholar
  10. Bradbury J, Vehrencamp S (1993) SingIt! A program for interactive playback on the Macintosh. Bioacoustics 5:308–310Google Scholar
  11. Bremond JC (1978) Acoustic competition between the song of the wren (Troglodytes troglodytes) and the songs of other species. Behaviour 65:89–098Google Scholar
  12. Bremond JC, Aubin T (1992) Temporal matching of song: an interactive playback experiment with territorial wrens (Troglodytes troglodytes). C R Acad Sci Ser III Sci Vie 314:37–42Google Scholar
  13. Brown CH, May BJ (1990) Sound localization and binaural processes. In: Berkley MA, Stebbins WC (eds) Comparative perception, vol I. Basic mechanisms. Wiley, New York, p 247Google Scholar
  14. Campbell DT, Stanley JC (1963) Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Rand McNally, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  15. Capranica RR, Frishkopf LS, Nevo E (1973) Encoding of geographic dialects in the auditory system of the cricket frog. Science 182:1272–1275PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Catchpole CK (1989) Pseudoreplication and external validity: playback experiments in avian bioacoustics. Trends Ecol Evol 4:286–287Google Scholar
  17. Chapman CA, Chapman LJ, Lefebvre L (1990) Spider monkey alarm calls: honest advertisement or warning kin? Anim Behav 39:197–198Google Scholar
  18. Chatfield C (1975) The analysis of time-series: theory and practice. Chapman and Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar
  19. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1988) Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 36:477–486Google Scholar
  20. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Silk JB (1995) The responses of female baboons (Papio cyanocephalus ursinus) to anomalous social interactions: evidence for causal reasoning? J Comp Psychol 109:134–141PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Cheon SM, Park SR (1995) Signal value of partial song (composed of 1 phrase unit) in great tits, Parus major: evidence from playback experiments. Korean J Zool 38:230–237Google Scholar
  22. Cormier SM, Hagman JD (eds) (1987) Transfer of learning: contemporary research and applications. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Dabelsteen T (1992) Interactive playback: a finely tuned response. In: McGregor (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 97Google Scholar
  24. Dabelsteen T, Pedersen SB (1991) A portable digital sound emitter for interactive playback of animal vocalizations. Bioacoustics 3:193–206Google Scholar
  25. Davis WJ (1986) Acoustic recognition in the belted kingfisher: cardiac response to playback vocalizations. Condor 88:505–512Google Scholar
  26. Diehl P (1992) Radiotelemetric measurements of heart rate in singing blackbirds (Turdus menila). J Ornithol 133:181–195Google Scholar
  27. Dooling RJ (1982) Auditory perception in birds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 1. Academic Press, New York, p 95Google Scholar
  28. Duellman WE, Pyles RA (1983) Acoustic resource partitioning in anuran communities. Copeia 1983: 639–649Google Scholar
  29. Enden ST (1972) An experimental analysis of the parameters of bird song eliciting species recognition. Behaviour 41:130–171Google Scholar
  30. Evans CS (1993) Recognition of the spectral characteristics of conspecific signals in ducklings: evidence for a simple perceptual process. Anim Behav 45:1071–1082Google Scholar
  31. Evans CS, Marler P (1991) On the use of video images as social stimuli in birds: audience effects on alarm calling. Anim Behav 41:17–26Google Scholar
  32. Falls JB (1981) Mapping territories with playback: an accurate census method for songbirds. Stud Avian Biol 6:86–91Google Scholar
  33. Falls JB (1992) Playback: a historical perspective. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 11Google Scholar
  34. Falls JB, Horn AG, Dickinson TE (1988) How western meadowlarks classify their songs: evidence from song matching. Anim Behav 36:579–585Google Scholar
  35. Farabaugh SM (1982) The ecological and social significance of duetting. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, Vol 2. Academic Press, New York, p 85Google Scholar
  36. Ficken RW, Ficken MS, Hailman JP (1974) Temporal pattern shifts to avoid acoustic interference in singing birds. Science 183:762–763PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Fisher J (1954) Evolution and bird sociality. In: Huxley J, Hardy AC, Ford EB (eds) Evolution as a process. George Allen and Unwin, London, p 71Google Scholar
  38. Fletcher H (1940) Auditory patterns. Rev Mod Phys 12:47–65Google Scholar
  39. Fletcher H, Munson WA (1937) Relation between loudness and masking. J Acoust Soc Am 9:1–10Google Scholar
  40. Garner RL (1892) Speech of monkeys. Webster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. Gerhardt HC (1991) Female mate choice in tree frogs: static and dynamic acoustic criteria. Anim Behav 42:615–635Google Scholar
  42. Gerhardt HC (1992) Conducting playback experiments and interpreting their results. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 59Google Scholar
  43. Gerhardt HC, Diekamp B, Ptacek M (1989) Inter-male spacing in choruses of the spring peeper, Pseudacris (Hyla) crucifer. Anim Behav 38:1012–1024Google Scholar
  44. Gilbert G, McGregor PK, Tyler G (1994) Vocal individuality as a census tool: practical considerations illustrated by a study of two rare species. J. Field Ornithol 65:335–348Google Scholar
  45. Gish SL, Morton ES (1981) Structural adaptations to local habitat acoustics in Carolina wren songs. Z Tierpsychol 56:74–84Google Scholar
  46. Godard R (1991) Long-term memory of individual neighbors in a migratory songbird. Nature 350:228–229Google Scholar
  47. Godard R (1993) Tit for tat among neighboring hooded warblers. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 33:45–50Google Scholar
  48. Gottlander K (1987) Variation in the song rate of the male pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca): causes and consequences. Anim Behav 35:1037–1043Google Scholar
  49. Greenberg R, Niven DK, Hopp SL, Boone C (1993) Frugivory and coexistence in a resident and a migratory vireo on the Yucatan Peninsula. Condor 95:990–999Google Scholar
  50. Grinnell J, Packer C, Pusey AE (1995) Cooperation in male lions: kinship, reciprocity or mutualism? Anim Behav 49:95–105Google Scholar
  51. Hambler C, Newing J, Hambler K (1994) Population monitoring for the flightless rail Dryolimnas cuvieri aldabranus. Bird Conserv Int 3:307–318Google Scholar
  52. Hansen P (1979) Vocal learning: its role in adapting sound structures to long distance propagation, and a hypothesis on its evolution. Anim Behav 27:1270–1271Google Scholar
  53. Hawkins AFA (1994) Conservation status and regional population estimates of the white-breasted mesite Mesitornis variegata, a rare Malagasy endemic. Bird Conserv Int 4:279–303Google Scholar
  54. Hodl W (1977) Call differences and calling site segregation in anuran species from central Amazonian floating meadows. Oecologia 28:351–363Google Scholar
  55. Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Marion J (1985) Olfactory discrimination of littermates in rats (Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 99:248–251PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Horn AG (1992) Field experiments on the perception of song types by birds. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 191Google Scholar
  57. Huribert SH (1984) Psuedoreplication and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecol Monogr 54:187–211Google Scholar
  58. Irwin FW (1958) An analysis of the concepts of discrimination and preference. Am J Psychol 71:152–163PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Jackson JE (1991) A users guide to principal components. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  60. James FC, McCulloch CE (1985) Data analysis and the design of experiments in ornithology. Curr Ornithol 2:1–63Google Scholar
  61. Klump GM, Gerhardt HC (1992) Mechanisms and function of call timing in male-male interactions in frogs. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 153Google Scholar
  62. Krebs JR, Ashcroft R, Van Orsdol K (1981) Song matching in the great tit Parus major. Anim Behav 29:918–923Google Scholar
  63. Kroodsma DE (1984) Songs of the alder flycatcher (Empidonax alnorum) and willow flycatcher (Empidonax traillii) are innate. Auk 101:13–24Google Scholar
  64. Kroodsma DE (1985) Development and use of two song forms by the eastern phoebe. Wilson Bull 97:21–29Google Scholar
  65. Kroodsma DE (1986) Design of song playback experiments. Auk 103:640–642Google Scholar
  66. Kroodsma DE (1989a) Suggested experimental designs for song play-backs. Anim Behav 37:600–609Google Scholar
  67. Kroodsma DE (1989b) Inappropriate experimental designs impede progress in bioacoustic research: a reply. Anim Behav 38:717–719Google Scholar
  68. Kroodsma DE (1989c) Male eastern phoebes (Sayornis phoebe; Tyrannidae, Passeriformes) fail to imitate songs. J Comp Psychol 103:227–232Google Scholar
  69. Kroodsma DE (1990) Using appropriate experimental designs for intended hypotheses in ‘song’ playbacks, with examples for testing effects of song repertoire sizes. Anim Behav 40:1138–1150Google Scholar
  70. Kroodsma DE, Konishi M (1991) A suboscine bird (Eastern Phoebe, Sayornis phoebe) develops normal song without auditory feedback Anim Behav 42:477–487Google Scholar
  71. Lambrechts MM, Dhondt AA (1995) Individual voice discrimination in birds. In: Power DM (ed) Current ornithology, vol 12. Plenum Press, New York, p 115Google Scholar
  72. Langbauer WR Jr, Payne KB, Charif RA, Rapaport L, Osborn F (1991) African elephants respond to distant playbacks of low-frequency conspecific calls. J Exp Biol 157:35–46Google Scholar
  73. Lehner PN (1979) Handbook of ethological methods. Garland Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  74. Lemon RE, Monette S, Roff D (1987) Song repertoires of American warblers (Parulinae): honest advertisement or assessment? Ethology 74:265–284Google Scholar
  75. Lewis C 1993) Bayesian methods for the analysis of variance. In: Keren G, Lewis C (eds) A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences. Earlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, p 233Google Scholar
  76. Lynch JF (1989) Distributions of overwintering Nearctic migrants in the Yucatan Peninsula, I: general patterns of occurrence. Condor 91:515–544Google Scholar
  77. Lynch JF, Morton ES, Van der Voort ME (1985) Habitat segregation between the sexes of wintering hooded warblers (Wilsonia citrina). Auk 102:714–721Google Scholar
  78. Mabey SE, Morton ES (1990) Demography and territorial behavior of wintering Kentucky warblers in Panama. In: Hagan JM, Johnston DW (eds) Ecology and conservation of Neotropical migrant landbirds. Smithsonian Press, Washington DC, p 329Google Scholar
  79. Machlis L, Dodd PWD, Fentress JC (1985) The pooling fallacy: problems arising when individuals contribute more than one observation to the data set. Z Tierpsychol 68:201–214Google Scholar
  80. Macedonia JM, Evans CS (1993) Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology 93:177–197Google Scholar
  81. Marier P, Mitani J (1988) Vocal communication in primates and birds: Parallels and contrasts. In: Todt D, Goedeking P, Symmes D (eds) Primate vocal communication. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, p 3Google Scholar
  82. Marier P, Pichert R (1984) species-universal microstructure in the learned song of the swamp sparrow (Melospiza georgiana). Anim Behav 32:673–689Google Scholar
  83. Marier P, Evans CS, Hauser M (1992) Animal signals: motivation, referential, or both? In: Papoušek H, Jürgens U, Papoušek M (eds) Nonverbal vocal communication: comparative and developmental approaches. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 66Google Scholar
  84. Marten K, Marier P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal communication. I. Temperate habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:271–90Google Scholar
  85. Martin G (1981) Avian vocalizations and the sound interference model of Roberts et al., Anim Behav 29:632–633Google Scholar
  86. Martin P, Bateson P (1986) Measuring behavior: an introductory guide. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  87. McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo. Anim Behav 47:379–387Google Scholar
  88. McGregor PK (ed) (1992a) Playback and Studies of Animal Communication. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  89. McGregor PK (1992b) Quantifying responses to playback: one, many, or composite multivariate measures? In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 79Google Scholar
  90. McGregor PK, Avery MI (1986) The unsung songs of great tits (Parus major): learning neighbor’s songs for discrimination. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:311–316Google Scholar
  91. McGregor PK, Falls RB (1984) The response of western meadowlarks (Stornella neglecta) to the playback of undegraded and degraded calls. Can J Zool 62:2125–2128Google Scholar
  92. McGregor PK, Horn AG (1992) Strophe length and response to playback in great tits. Anim Behav 43:667–676Google Scholar
  93. McGregor PK, Krebs JR (1984) Sound degradation as a distance cue in great tit (Parus major) song. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:49–56Google Scholar
  94. McGregor PK, Ranft RD (1994) Equipment for sound analysis and playback: a survey. Bioacoustics 6:83–86Google Scholar
  95. McGregor PK, Krebs JR, Ratcliffe LM (1983) The reaction of great tits (Parus major) to playback of degraded and undegraded songs: the effect of familiarity with the stimulus song type. Auk 100:898–906Google Scholar
  96. McGregor PK, Catchpole CK, Dabelsteen T, et al. (1992) Design of playback experiments: the Thornbridge Hall NATO ARW consensus. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 1Google Scholar
  97. Michelsen A (1978) Sound reception in different environments. In: Ali MA (ed) Sensory ecology, review and perspectives. Plenum Press, New York, p 345Google Scholar
  98. Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 109:17–34Google Scholar
  99. Morton ES (1982) Grading, discreetness, redundancy, and motivation-structural rules. In: Kroodsma DK, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol I. Academic Press, New York, p 183Google Scholar
  100. Morton ES (1986) Predictions from the ranging hypothesis for the evolution of long distance signals in birds. Behaviour 99:65–86Google Scholar
  101. Morton ES (1990) Habitat segregation by sex in the hooded warbler: experiments on proximate causation and discussion of its evolution. Am Nat 135:319–333Google Scholar
  102. Morton ES (1996) A comparison of vocal behavior among tropical and temperate passerine birds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, p 258Google Scholar
  103. Morton ES, Young K (1986) A previously undescribed method of song matching in a species with a single song “type”, the Kentucky warbler (Oporornis formosus). Ethology 73:334–342Google Scholar
  104. Morton ES, Gish SL, Van der Voort M (1986) On the learning of degraded and undegraded songs in the Carolina wren. Anim Behav 34:815–820Google Scholar
  105. Morton ES, Van der Voort M, Greenberg R (1993) How a warbler chooses its habitat: field support for laboratory experiments. Anim Behav 46:47–53Google Scholar
  106. Narins PM, Capranica RR (1976) Sexual differences in the auditory system of the treefrog Eleutherodactylus coqui. Science 192:378–380PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Narins PM, Smith SL (1986) Clinal variation in anuran advertisement calls: basis for acoustic isolation? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:135–41Google Scholar
  108. Nelson DA (1989) Song frequency as a cue for recognition of species and individuals in the field sparrow (Spizella pusilla). J Comp Psychol 103:171–176PubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Nelson DA, Marler P (1990) The perception of birdsong and an ecological concept of signal space. In: Stebbins WC, Berkeley MA (eds) Comparative perception vol II. Complex signals. Wiley, New York, p 443Google Scholar
  110. Nelson DA, Marier P (1994) Selection-based learning in bird song development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:10498–10501PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Nowicki S, Mitani JC, Nelson DA, Marier P (1989) The communicative significance of tonality in birdsong: responses to songs produced in helium. Bioacoustics 2:35–46Google Scholar
  112. Otter K, Njegovan M, Naugler C, Fotheringham J, Ratcliffe L (1993) A simple technique for interactive playback experiments using a Macintosh Powerbook computer. Bioacoustics 5:303–307Google Scholar
  113. Owren MJ, Seyfarth RM, Hopp SL (1992) Categorical vocal signaling in nonhuman primates. In: Papoušek H, Jürgens U, Papoušek M (eds) Nonverbal vocal communication: comparative and developmental approaches. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 102Google Scholar
  114. Pallett JR, Passmore NI (1988) The significance of multi-note advertisement calls in the reed frog, Hyperolius tuberilinguis. Bioacoustics 1:13–24Google Scholar
  115. Payne RB (1986) Bird songs and avian systematics. Curr Ornithol 3:87–126Google Scholar
  116. Petrinovich L, Patterson TL (1980) Field studies of habituation. III. Playback contingent on the response of the white-crowned sparrow. Anim Behav 28:742–751Google Scholar
  117. Popp JW, Ficken RW (1987) Effects of non-specific singing on the song of the ovenbird. Bird Behav 7:22–26Google Scholar
  118. Price K (1994) Center-edge effect in red squirrels: evidence from playback experiments. J Mammal 75:545–548Google Scholar
  119. Pridmore-Brown DC, Ingard U (1955) Sound propagation into the shadow zone in a temperature-stratified atmosphere above a plane boundary. J Acoust Soc Am 27:36–42Google Scholar
  120. Radesäter T, Jakobsson S, Andbjer N, Bylin A, Nyström K (1987) Song rate and pair formation in the willow warbler, Phylloscopus trochilus. Anim Behav 35:1645–1651Google Scholar
  121. Reid ML (1987) Costliness and reliability in the singing vigour of Ipswich sparrows. Anim Behav 35:1735–1743Google Scholar
  122. Rendali D, Rodman PS, Emond RE (1996) Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Anim Behav 51:1007–1015Google Scholar
  123. Richards DG (1981) Estimation of distance of singing conspecifics by the Carolina wren. Auk 98:127–133Google Scholar
  124. Richards DG, Wiley RH (1980) Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest: implications for animal communication. Am Nat 115:381–399Google Scholar
  125. Roberts J, Hunter ML, Kacelnik A (1981) The ground effect and acoustic communication. Anim Behav 29:633–634Google Scholar
  126. Robinson SR, Terborgh J (1995) Interspecific aggression and habitat selection by Amazonian birds. J Anim Ecol 64:1–11Google Scholar
  127. Rowland WJ, Bolyard KJ, Jenkins JJ, Fowler J (1995) Video playback experiments on stickleback mate choice: female motivation and attentiveness to male color cues. Anim Behav 49:1559–1567Google Scholar
  128. Rowley I, Russell E (1993) The purple-crowned fairy-wren Malurus coronatus: II. Breeding biology, social organization, demography and management. Emu 93:235–250Google Scholar
  129. Rushe RH, Gottman JM (1993) Essentials in the design and analysis of time-series experiments In: Keren G, Lewis C (eds) A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences: statistical issues. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, p 493Google Scholar
  130. Ryan MJ, Brenowitz EA (1985) The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am Nat 126:87–100Google Scholar
  131. Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1983a) An experimental study of acoustic interference between two species of neotropical treefrogs. Anim Behav 31:181–190Google Scholar
  132. Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1983b) The influence of background noise on the behavior of a neotropical treefrog, Hyla ebraccata. Herpetologia 39:121–125Google Scholar
  133. Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1984a) Vocal behavior of the neotropical treefrog Hyla phlebodes. Herpetologia 40:452–463Google Scholar
  134. Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1984b) Interspecific acoustic interactions of the neotropical treefrog Hyla ebraccata. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 14:211–224Google Scholar
  135. Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1985) Intra- and interspecific vocal behavior of the neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala. Copeia 1985:27–38Google Scholar
  136. Searcy WA (1989) Pseudoreplication, external validity and the design of playback experiments. Anim Behav 38:715–717Google Scholar
  137. Searcy WA (1992) Measuring responses of female birds to male song. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, p 175Google Scholar
  138. Searcy WA, Yasukawa K (1996) Song and female choice. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, p 454Google Scholar
  139. Searcy WA, Coffman S, Raikow DF (1994) Habituation, recovery and the similarity of song types within repertoires in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus) (Aves, Emberizidae). Ethology 98: 38–49Google Scholar
  140. Searcy WA, Podos J, Peters S, Nowicki S (1995) Discrimination of song types and variants in song sparrows. Anim Behav 49:1219–1226Google Scholar
  141. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1990) The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species’ alarm calls. Anim Behav 40:754–764Google Scholar
  142. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence for predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210:801–803PubMedGoogle Scholar
  143. Shy E, Morton ES (1986a) Adaptation of amplitude structure of songs to propagation in field habitat in song sparrows. Ethology 72:77–84Google Scholar
  144. Shy E, Morton ES (1986b) The role of distance, familiarity, and time of day in Carolina wrens responses to conspecific songs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:393–400Google Scholar
  145. Sliwa A, Sherry TW (1992) Surveying winter warbler populations in Jamaica: point counts with and without broadcast vocalizations. Condor 94:924–936Google Scholar
  146. Smith WJ (1996) Using interactive playback to study how songs and singing contribute to communication about behavior. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p 377Google Scholar
  147. Smith WJ, Smith AM (1992) Behavioral information provided by two song forms of the eastern kingbird, Tyrannus tyrannus. Behaviour 120:90–102Google Scholar
  148. Snowdon C, Hodun A, Rosenberger A, Coimbra-Filho A (1986) Long-call structure and its relation to taxonomy in lion tamarins. Am J Primatol 11:253–262Google Scholar
  149. Sorjonen J (1983) Transmission of the two most characteristic phrases of the song of the thrush nightingale (Lucinia lucinia) in different environmental conditions. Ornis Scand 14:278–288Google Scholar
  150. Stevens DA (1975) Laboratory methods for obtaining olfactory discrimination in rodents. In: Moulton DG, Turk A, Johnston JW Jr (eds) Methods in olfactory research. Academic Press, New York, p 375Google Scholar
  151. Stoddard PK (1990) Audio computers: theory of operation and guidelines for selection of systems and components. Bioacoustics 2:217–239Google Scholar
  152. Stoddard PK (1996) Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines. In: Koodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca p 356Google Scholar
  153. Stoddard PK, Beecher MD, Campbell SE, Horning CL (1992) Song-type matching in the song sparrow. Can J Zool 70:1440–1444Google Scholar
  154. Strain JG, Mumme RL (1988) Effects of food supplementation, song playback, and temperature on vocal territorial behavior of Carolina wrens. Auk 105:11–16Google Scholar
  155. Struhsaker T (1967) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In: Altmann S (ed) Social communication among primates. Chicago University Press, Chicago, p 281Google Scholar
  156. Symmes D, Biben M (1986) Maternal recognition of individual infant squirrel monkeys from isolation call playbacks. Am J Primatol 9:39–46Google Scholar
  157. Thompson R, Spencer D (1966) Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol Rev 73:16–43PubMedGoogle Scholar
  158. Timm NM (1993) Manova and Mancova: an overview. In: Keren G, Lewis C (eds) A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, p 129Google Scholar
  159. Vallet E, Kreutzer M (1995) Female canaries are sexually responsive to special song phrases. Anim Behav 49:1603–1610Google Scholar
  160. Villard MA, Merriam G, Maurer BA (1995) Dynamics in subdivided populations of neotropical migratory birds in a fragmented temperate forest. Ecology 76:27–40Google Scholar
  161. Waser PM, Brown CH (1984) Is there a “sound window” for primate communications? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 15:73–76Google Scholar
  162. Waser PM, Brown CH (1986) Habitat acoustics and primate communication. Am J Primatol 10:135–54Google Scholar
  163. Waser PM, Waser MS (1977) Experimental studies of primate vocalization: specializations for long-distance propagation. Z Tierpsychol 43:239–263Google Scholar
  164. Wasserman FE (1977) Intraspecific acoustical interference in the white-throated sparrow, (Zonotrichia albicollis). Anim Behav 25:949–952Google Scholar
  165. Weary DM, Kramer DL (1995) Response of eastern chipmunks to conspecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 49:81–93Google Scholar
  166. Weeden JS, Falls JB (1959) Differential responses of male ovenbirds to recorded songs of neighboring and more distant individuals. Auk 76:343–351Google Scholar
  167. Whitehead JM (1987) Vocally mediated reciprocity between neighboring groups of mantled howling monkeys, Aloutta palliata palliata. Anim Behav 35:1615–1627Google Scholar
  168. Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  169. Winkler RL (1993) Bayesian statistics: an overview. In: Keren G, Lewis C (eds) A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, p 201Google Scholar
  170. Wooller RD (1978) Individual vocal recognition in the kittiwake gull, Rissa tridactyla. Z Tierpsychol 48:68–86Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • S. L. Hopp
  • E. S. Morton

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations