Cognitive Therapy and the Analysis of Meaning Structures

  • L.-G. Lundh

Abstract

Each person has his own way of experiencing the world. How a given situation is perceived and interpreted varies from one person to another, and may also vary from one occasion to another within the same person. Different psychological theories have tried to approach these basic facts in terms of various concepts. According to Piaget (1951), Neisser (1976) and others, each person has his own “schemata”, in terms of which he or she assimilates the world. According to George Kelly (1955), we all have our “personal constructs” which channel our psychological processes. Other psychologists speak in this connection of “mental structures” (e.g. Mandler 1975), “semantic networks” (e.g. Anderson and Bower 1973), “memory structures” (Posner 1973), and “interassociated nodes in long-term memory” (Shiffrin and Schneider 1977).

Keywords

Placebo Depression Cage Schizophrenia Assimilation 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson LY, Seligman MEP, Teasdale JD (1978) Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol 87: 49–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander F, French TM (1946) Psychoanalytic therapy. Ronald, New York.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson CA (1983) Motivational and performance deficits in interpersonal settings: the effect of attributional style. J Pers Soc Psychol 45: 1136–1147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson CA, Horowitz LM, French R (1983) Attributional style of lonely and depressed people. J Pers Soc Psychol 45: 127–136.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson JR, Bower GH (1973) Human associative memory. Winston, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  6. Bandura A (1977) Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs.Google Scholar
  7. Bandura A (1982) Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am Psychol 37: 122–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bateson G, Jackson DD, Haley J, Weakland JH (1956) Towards a theory of schizophrenia. Behav Sci 1: 251–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Beck AT (1967) Depression. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  10. Beck AT (1976) Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. International Universities Press, New York.Google Scholar
  11. Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, Emery G (1979) Cognitive therapy of depression. Guilford, New York.Google Scholar
  12. Bower GH (1981) Mood and memory. Am Psychol 36: 129–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bruner JS (1957) On perceptual readiness. Psychol Rev 64: 123–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chomsky N (1980) Rules and representations. Columbia University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  15. Craik KJW (1943) The nature of explanation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  16. Cramer B, Stern D (1986) Mother-infant psychotherapy: objective and subjective changes. Paper presented at the third world congress on infant psychiatry and allied disciplines.Google Scholar
  17. Flavell JH (1977) Cognitive development. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs.Google Scholar
  18. Fodor JA (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  19. Frank JD (1973) Persuasion and healing. John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore.Google Scholar
  20. Frank JD (1983) The placebo is psychotherapy. Behav Brain Sci 6: 291–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Frankl VE (1978) The unheard cry for meaning. Simon and Schuster, New York.Google Scholar
  22. Freud S (1900) The interpretation of dreams. Hogarth, London.Google Scholar
  23. Freud S (1914) Remembering, repeating and working-through, standard edn, vol XII. Hogarth, London, p 147.Google Scholar
  24. Gallimore RG, Turner JL (1977) Contemporary studies of placebo phenomena. In: Jarvik ME (ed) Psychopharmacology in the practice of medicine. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.Google Scholar
  25. Gill MM (1982) Analysis of transference, vol I. International Universities Press, New York.Google Scholar
  26. Janoff-Bulman R (1979) Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: inquiries into depression and rape. J Pers Soc Psychol 37: 1798–1809.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kelly GA (1955) A theory of personality. The psychology of personal constructs. Morton, New York.Google Scholar
  28. Klüwer H (1933) Behavioral mechanisms in monkeys. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  29. Kohut H (1984) How does analysis cure? University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  30. Lakatos I (1970) Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In: Lakatos I Musgrave A (ed) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  31. Lashley KS (1942) The problem of cerebral organization in vision. In: Pribram K (ed) Brain and behavior. II. Perception and action. Penguin, Harmondsworth.Google Scholar
  32. Lewin K (1951) Field theory in social science. Harper, New York.Google Scholar
  33. Loftus EF (1980) Memory. Addison-Wesley, Reading.Google Scholar
  34. Lundh LG (1979) Introspection, consciousness and human information processing. Scand J Psychol 20: 223–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lundh LG (1983) Mind and meaning. Towards a theory of the human mind considered as a system of meaning structures. Acta Univ Ups Stud Psychol Ups 10:.Google Scholar
  36. Lundh LG (1987) Placebo, belief and health. A cognitive-emotional model. Scand J Psychol 28: 128–143.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McDougall W (1936) An introduction to social psychology. Methuen, London.Google Scholar
  38. Macnamara J (1972) Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. Psychol Rev 79: 1–13.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mandler G (1975) Mind and emotion. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  40. Neisser U (1976) Cognition and reality. Freeman, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  41. Nelson K (1974) Concept, word and sentence. Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychol Rev 81: 267–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned reflexes. Dover, New York.Google Scholar
  43. Pavlov IP (1928) Lectures on conditioned reflexes: the higher nervous activity (behaviour) of animals, vol I. Lawrence and Wishart, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Penfield W (1975) The mystery of the mind. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  45. Piaget J (1936) The origin of intelligence in the child. Penguin, Harmondsworth, Middlesex.Google Scholar
  46. Piaget J (1947) The psychology of intelligence. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Piaget J (1951) Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
  48. Piaget J, Inhelder B (1968) Memory and intelligence. Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  49. Popper KR, Eccles JC (1977) The self and its brain. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York.Google Scholar
  50. Posner MI (1973) Cognition: an introduction. Scott, Foresman Glenview.Google Scholar
  51. Prioleau L, Murdock M, Brody N (1983) An analysis of psychotherapy versus placebo studies. Behav Brain Sci 6: 275–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Seligman MEP, Abramson LY, Semmel A, von Baeyer C (1979) Depressive attributional style. J Abnorm Psychol 88: 242–247.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Shapiro AK, Morris LA (1978) The placebo effect in medical and psychological therapies. In. Garfield SL, Bergin AE (ed) Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: an empirical analysis. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  54. Sherif CW (1980) Social values, attitudes, and the involvement of the self. In: Howe HE (ed) Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln.Google Scholar
  55. Shiffrin RM, Schneider W (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychol Rev 84: 129–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Skinner BF (1935) The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. J Gen Psychol 12: 40–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Skinner BF (1953) Science and human behavior. Free, New York.Google Scholar
  58. Snyder M, Swann W (1976) When actions reflect attitudes: the politics of impression management. J Pers Soc Psychol 34: 1034–1042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Snyder M, Swann W (1978) Hypothesis-testing processes in social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 36: 1202–1212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sokolov EN (1958) Perception and the conditioned reflex. Macmillan, New York.Google Scholar
  61. Teasdale JD (1985) Psychological treatments for depression: how do they work? Behav Res Ther 23: 157–165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tolman EC (1932) Purposive behavior in animals and men. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.Google Scholar
  63. Tolman EC (1949) There is more than one kind of learning. Psychol Rev 56: 144–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W (ed) Organization of memory. Academic, New York.Google Scholar
  65. von Bertalanffy L (1968) General system theory. Braziller, New York.Google Scholar
  66. Watts AW (1957) The way of zen. Penguin, Harmondsworth.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • L.-G. Lundh
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Clinical PsychologyUniversity of UppsalaUppsalaSweden

Personalised recommendations