Advertisement

EEG-Studies in Musical Perception and Performances

  • H. Petsche
  • H. Pockberger
  • P. Rappelsberger

Abstract

In the past decade, electroencephalography has turned out to become a useful tool for the understanding of human cognition. The application of statistical and analytical methods to the electrical signals produced by the brain has brought some insight into the meaning of electrical events accompanying the conceptual processing of sensory perception and volition. There is an enormous proliferation of psychological experiments to study eventrelated responses, which are known to contain some information on mental processes. The same holds true for the still increasing sophistication of the experimental paradigms. Nevertheless, it seems doubtful, whether all these psychological tasks, which are often meaningless for the persons performing them, are capable of revealing major relations between thinking processes and EEG phenomena. As Ulric Neisser rightly states: “A satisfactory theory of human cognition can hardly be established by experiments that provide inexperienced subjects with brief opportunities to perform novel and meaningless tasks” (1976).

Keywords

Median Frequency Alpha Power Absolute Power Theta Power Musical Perception 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bartlett, F. O: Remembering. Cambridge University Press, 1932.Google Scholar
  2. Beaumont, J. G., A. R. Mayes, M. D. Rugg: Asymmetry in EEG alpha coherence and power: effects of task and sex. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. 45, 393–401 (1978).Google Scholar
  3. Berger, H.: Über das Elektroenzephalogramm des Menschen. Arch. f. Psychiat. 87, 527–570 (1929).Google Scholar
  4. Bever, Th. G.: Cerebral asymmetries in humans are due to the differentiation of two incompatible processes: holistic and analytic. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 263, 251–262 (1975).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bever, T. G., R. J. Chiarello: Cerebral dominance in musicians and nonmusicians. Science 185, 537–539 (1974).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Busk, J., G. C. Galbraith: EEG correlates of visual-motor practice in man. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. 38, 415–422 (1975).Google Scholar
  7. Duffy, F. H., P. H. Bartels, J. L. Burchfiel: Significance probability mapping: an aid in the topographic analysis of brain electrical activity. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. 51, 455–462 (1981).Google Scholar
  8. Gasser, T., P. Bächer, J. Mocks: Transformations towards the normal distribution of broad band spectral parameters of the EEG. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. 53, 119–124(1982).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gombrich, E. H.(Hrsg.): Ornament und Kunst. Klett-Cotta, Stuttgart 1982.Google Scholar
  10. Gordon, H. W.: Hemispheric asymmetry for dichotically-presented chords in musicians and nonmusicians, males and females. Acta Psychol. 42, 383–395 (1978).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hirshkowitz, M., J. Earle, E. Paley: EEG asymmetry in musicians and nonmusicians: a study of hemispheric specialization. Neurophysiologia 16, 125–128 (1978).Google Scholar
  12. John, E. R., R. W. Thatcher, E. R. John (Eds.): Foundations of Cognitive Processes (Functional Neuroscience, Vol. I). John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1977.Google Scholar
  13. Kimura, D.: Left-right differences in the perception of melodies. Quaterly J. Exp. Psychol. 16, 355–358 (1964).Google Scholar
  14. Lindsley, D. B.: Attention, consciousness, sleep and wakefulness. In: J. Field, H. W. Magoun, V. E. Hall (Eds.): Handbook of Physiology, Vol.3, Washington, D. C, Am. Physiol. Soc, 1960.Google Scholar
  15. McKee, G., B. Humphrey, D. W. McAdam: Scaled lateralization of alpha activity during linguistic and musical tasks. Psychophysiology 10, 441–443 (1973).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Milner, B.: Laterality effects in audition. In: V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.): Interhemi-spheric Relations and Cerebral Dominance, Baltimore, Johns Hopkins Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  17. Moore, W. H. jr.: Alpha hemispheric asymmetry of males and females on verbal and nonverbal tasks: some preliminary results. Cortex 15, 321–326 (1979).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Morais, J., I. Peretz, M. Gudanski: Ear asymmetry for chord recognition in musicians and nonmusicians. Neuropsychologia 20, 351–354 (1982).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Neisser, U.: Cognition and reality. Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. Freeman and Co., San Francisco 1976.Google Scholar
  20. Piaget, J. (Ed.): The construction of reality in the child. New York, Basic Books, 1954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Rogers, L. J., D. O. Walter: Methods for findings single generators, with application to auditory driving of the human EEG by complex stimuli. J. Neurosci. Methods 4, 257–265 (1981).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Shaw, J. C, K. O’Connor, C. Ongley: EEG coherence as a measure of cerebral functional organisation. In: M. A. B. Brazier and H. Petsche (Eds.): Architectonics of the Cerebral Cortex. Raven Press, New York 1978, pp. 245–256.Google Scholar
  23. Schwartz, G. E., R. J. Davidson, F. Maer, E. Bromfield: Patterns of hemispheric dominance in musical, emotional, verbal and spatial tasks. (Abstract). Psychophysiology 4, 227 (1974).Google Scholar
  24. Sklar, B., J. Hanley, W. W. Simmons: An EEG experiment aimed toward identifying dyslexic children. Nature (Lond.), 24, 414–416 (1972).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Walker, J. L.: Alpha EEG correlates of performance on a music recognition task. Physiol. Psychol. 8, 417–420 (1980).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1987

Authors and Affiliations

  • H. Petsche
  • H. Pockberger
  • P. Rappelsberger

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations