Advertisement

How Insects Detect and Avoid Bats

  • Lee A. Miller

Abstract

From his field studies, Roeder (1962) predicted that green lacewings, like moths, might possess anti-bat behaviors. As a result I started studying green lacewings. My first meeting with Ken Roeder took place in the introductory biology laboratory at Harvard, where I was a teaching assistant. He came over from Tufts to see if it was really true that someone was studying hearing in green lacewings! I had not considered contacting him before I thought I had something to tell him. This was my mistake. Any aspect of bat—insect interactions, be it morphological, physiological, behavioral or evolutionary, was enough to fire his imagination. We discussed details, but it became apparent that, for Roeder, hearing was just one facet in the physiology of behavior. His multifarious approach to moths and ultrasound illustrated the importance of behavior when studying neural mechanisms. After all, selection pressure on the behavior shapes the nervous system; not the reverse.

Keywords

Flight Muscle Noctuid Moth Afferent Response Green Lacewing Eptesicus Fuscus 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Albers VM (1965) Underwater acoustics handbook I I. Pennsylvania State Univ Press, Univ ParkGoogle Scholar
  2. Camhi JM, Tom W (1978) The escape behavior of the cockroach Periplaneta americana I. Turning response to wind puffs. J Comp Physiol 128: 193–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dunning DC (1968) Warning sounds of moths. Z Tierpsychol 25: 129–138PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Eaton RC, Lavender WA, Wieland CM (1981) Identification of Mauthner-initiated response patterns in goldfish: evidence from simulatneous cinematography and electrophysiology. J Comp Physiol 144: 521–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Fullard JH, Fenton MB (1977) Acoustic and behavioural analyses of the sounds produced by some species of Nearctic Arctiidae (Lepidoptera). Can J Zool 55: 1213–1224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fullard JH, Fenton MB, Simmons JA (1979) Jamming bat echolocation: the clicks of arctiid moths. Can J Zool 57: 647–649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Griffin DR (1958) Listening in the dark. Yale Univ Press, New Haven (reprinted 1974 Dover Publications, New York)Google Scholar
  8. Kick SA (1982) Target-detection by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Comp Physiol 145: 431–435CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kondoh Y, Obara Y (1982) Anatomy of motorneurones innervating mesothoracic indirect flight muscles in the silkmoth, Bombyx mori. J Exp Biol 98: 23–37Google Scholar
  10. Krasne FB, Wine JJ (1977) Control of crayfish escape behavior. In: Hoyle G (ed) Identified neurons and behavior of arthropods. Plenum Press, New York London, pp 275–292Google Scholar
  11. Miller LA (1970) Structure of the green lacewing tympanal organ (Chrysopa carnea, Neuroptera). J Morphol 131: 359–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Miller LA (1971) Physiological responses of green lacewings (Chrysopa, Neuroptera) to ultrasound. J Insect Physiol 17: 491–5 06Google Scholar
  13. Miller LA (1982) The orientation and evasive behavior of insects to bat cries. In: Addink ADF, Spronk N (eds) Exogenous and endogenous influences on metabolic and neural control, vol 1. Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 393–405Google Scholar
  14. Miller LA, Olesen J (1979) Avoidance behavior in green lacewings I. Behavior of free-flying green lacewings to hunting bats and to ultrasound. J Comp Physiol 131: 113–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Máhl B, Miller LA (1976) Ultrasonic clicks produced by the peacock butterfly: a possible bat-repellent mechanism. J Exp Biol 64: 639–644Google Scholar
  16. Olesen J, Miller LA (1979) Avoidance behavior in green lacewings II. Flight muscle activity. J Comp Physiol 131: 121–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Roeder KD (1962) The behaviour of free-flying moths in the presence of artificial ultrasonic pulses. Anim Behav 10: 300–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Roeder KD (1967) Nerve cells and insect behavior, rev edn. Harvard Univ Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  19. Roeder KD (1972) Acoustic and mechanical sensitivity of the distal lob of the pilifer in choerocampine hawkmoths. J Insect Physiol 18: 1249–1264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Roeder KD (1974a) Acoustic sensory responses and possible bat-evasion tactics of certain moths. Proc Can Soc Zool Ann Meeting 1974: 71–78Google Scholar
  21. Roeder KD (1974b) Some neuronal mechanisms of simple behavior. In: Lehrman DS, Rosenblatt JD, Hinde RA, Shaw E (eds) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 5. Academic Press, New York London, pp 1–46Google Scholar
  22. Roeder KD (1975) Neural factors and evitability in insect behavior. J Exp Zool 194: 75–88PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Roeder KD, Dunning DC (1968) Moth sounds and the insect-catching behavior of bats. Science 147: 173–174Google Scholar
  24. Roeder KD, Treat AE (1957) Ultrasonic reception by the tympanic organ of noctuid moths. J Exp Zool 134: 127–158PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Roeder KD, Treat AE (1962) The acoustic detection of bats by moths. Proc 11th Entomol Congr 3: 7–11Google Scholar
  26. Roeder KD, Treat AE, Vande Berg JS (1968) Auditory sense in certain hawkmoths. Science 159: 331–333PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Schnitzler HU, Henson OW (1980) Performance of airborne animal sonar systems: I. Microchiroptera. In: Busnel RG, Fish JF (eds) Animal sonar systems. Pelnum Press, New York London, pp 109–195Google Scholar
  28. Schoilten P, Larsen ON, Michelsen A (1981) Mechanical time resolution in same insect ears. I. Impulse responses and time constants. J Comp Physiol 143: 289–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Simmons JA (1980) The processing of sonar echoes by bats. In: Busnel RG, Fish JF (eds) Animal sonar systems. Plenum Press, New York London, pp 695–714Google Scholar
  30. Simmons JA, Stein RA (1980) Acoustic imaging in bat sonar: Echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation. J Comp Physiol 135: 61–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Stapells DR, Picton TW, Smith AD (1982) Normal hearing thresholds for clicks. J Acoust Soc Am 72: 74–79PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Surlykke A, Miller LA (1982) Central branchings of three sensory axons from a moth ear (Agrotis segetum, Noctuidae). J Insect Physiol 28: 357–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Werner TK (1981) Responses of nonflying moths to ultrasound: the threat of gleaning bats. Can J Zool 59: 525–529CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lee A. Miller
    • 1
  1. 1.Biologisk InstitutOdense UniversitetOdense MDenmark

Personalised recommendations