Skip to main content

Learning: Statistical Mechanisms in Language Acquisition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Language Phenomenon

Part of the book series: The Frontiers Collection ((FRONTCOLL))

Abstract

The grammatical structure of human languages is extremely complex, yet children master this complexity with apparent ease. One explanation is that we come to the task of acquisition equipped with knowledge about the possible grammatical structures of human languages—so-called “Universal Grammar”. An alternative is that grammatical patterns are abstracted from the input via a process of identifying reoccurring patterns and using that information to form grammatical generalizations. This statistical learning hypothesis receives support from computational research, which has revealed that even low level statistics based on adjacent word co-occurrences yield grammatically relevant information. Moreover, even as adults, our knowledge and usage of grammatical patterns is often graded and probabilistic, and in ways which directly reflect the statistical makeup of the language we experience. The current chapter explores such evidence and concludes that statistical learning mechanisms play a critical role in acquisition, whilst acknowledging holes in our current knowledge, particularly with respect to the learning of ‘higher level’ syntactic behaviours. Throughout, I emphasize that although a statistical approach is traditionally associated with a strongly empiricist position, specific accounts make specific claims about the nature of the learner, both in terms of learning mechanisms and the information that is primitive to the learning system. In particular, working models which construct grammatical generalizations often assume inbuilt semantic abstractions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    More accurately, this word is usually categorized as a “co-verb”. Li and Thompson (1974) (cited in DeLancey 2005) argue that co-verbs are graded in how syntactically “verb like” they are.

  2. 2.

    “Subject-hood” is itself defined in terms of the position that the NP holds within the hierarchical structure.

  3. 3.

    The nineteenth century assumption that non-Western languages are more grammatically primitive is long discredited. This is not to say that particular languages may lack particular grammatical devices. To take an extreme example, Pirahã, a language spoken by a tribe of around a hundred people in a remote area of the Amazon, has been reported (controversially—e.g. Nevins et al. 2009) to lack certain grammatical structures previously thought to be universal. Nevertheless, Everett points out that Pirahã employs a highly complex, intricate grammatical system: “No one should draw the conclusion from this paper that the Pirahã language is in any way ‘primitive’. It has the most complex verbal morphology I am aware of and a strikingly complex prosodic system.” (footnote in Everett 2005).

  4. 4.

    Languages may have different dialects, but there is internal agreement for speakers of that dialect.

  5. 5.

    A variety of techniques exist for assessing whether pre-verbal infants distinguish different types of stimuli. Saffran et al. (1996) used preferential listening where infants indicate their interest in some aural stimuli by looking at a light which they associate with that stimuli. Longer looking times are taken to indicate greater interest in the stimuli. Saffran et al. (1996) found that, after exposure to the nonsense syllable stream, infants showing longer looking times for part-word test items than for word test items (the stimuli were played repeatedly until the infant looked away from the light). The interpretation is that they found the part-words to be more novel and therefore more interesting.

  6. 6.

    This under-estimates, rather than over-estimates, the quantity of language to which a child is likely to be exposed. Hart and Risely (1995) estimate that working class children hear an average of 6 million words per year.

  7. 7.

    Both of these themes have been emphasized by other researchers. See Newport and Aslin (2000) for a statistical learning approach which strongly emphasizes the importance of innate constraints on learning. See Elman et al. (1996) for a connectionist approach to the issue of “innateness” in terms of the architectural make up of networks in different domains; See Seidenberg (1997) for a discussion of the relationship between statistical effects in language learning and language processing.

  8. 8.

    Languages may make much more extensive use of productive morphology than English. For example, in many languages (e.g. many of the Eskimoan languages) entire nouns may be attached to the verb-stem as dependent morphemes, rather than appearing as separate words within the sentence (a phenomenon known as “noun incorporation”).

  9. 9.

    Some more recent versions of this theory allow that at least some regular forms also be stored as whole forms (Pinker 1999; Pinker and Ullman 2002).

  10. 10.

    Although connectionist models are neurally inspired, there is no claim that they constitute a biologically plausible model of neural circuitry.

  11. 11.

    Later models had more complex architectures, including layers of hidden units between the input and output units, and used different learning algorithms.

  12. 12.

    All connectionist models require an error signal to drive learning. The models learn by predicting outputs for given inputs (early on predictions are random guesses), receiving feedback as to what the correct response should be, and then updating the “weights” (which drive the predictions) accordingly. For models which map between phonology and semantics, we are envisioning a child who implicitly compares the sound she would have expected for a given meaning with the one she is hearing, and the meaning she would have expected for a given form with the one that is currently implied.

  13. 13.

    Some researchers have argued that the most frequent form is not always the one that acts as the regular rule (e.g. Marcus et al. 1995). However in such cases the variety of types may be important. Plunkett and Nakisa (1997) demonstrate that a pattern which is not the most frequent can become the most productive in a connectionist model provided that the set of words to which the pattern applies are more dissimilar to each other than is the case for the sets of words associated with alternative patterns. Capturing such variability relies on the use of models with a more complex architecture, including a layer of hidden units between input and output mappings.

  14. 14.

    The critical factor appears to be whether past-tense forms are potentially decomposable, rather than whether the relationship between stem and past tense is regular. For example, slept is traditionally irregular but is nevertheless decomposable into slep \(+\) /t/ (note that this fits Fig. 4.1 as /p/ is voiceless) and it seems to be processed akin to regulars rather than irregulars (Joanisse and Seidenberg 2005).

  15. 15.

    This is a standard methodology for assessing infant preferences for a particular visual stimuli.

  16. 16.

    The data from the Gertner et al. (2006) experiments were not specifically modelled in Chang et al. (2006) but the result is generally consistent with the model’s account.

  17. 17.

    Ultimately we need an account of language learning and language change which explains why word order and case marking are so prevalent as means of encoding thematic information. However from the perspective of learning, the account must also be sufficiently flexible to explain the learning of other additional or alternative devices. For example, sign languages may also employ the modality specific device of directing signs with the signing space (e.g. moving a GIVE gesture towards a particular person to indicate that they are the recipient).

  18. 18.

    Since I have found that people outside of this discipline (particularly middle class academically minded parents, accustomed to explicitly correcting their children’s grammar) have difficulty accepting this point, it is worth highlighting. To further see that parental correction does not account for our knowledge of verb syntax, consider that many of the verbs which are ungrammatical in this construction are Latinate verbs (e.g. donate). It seems unlikely that such verbs are widely used (and therefore corrected) in childhood, yet we all known their syntactic restrictions.

  19. 19.

    Although Everett (2005) controversially claims that Pirahã lacks the ability to encode recursion, a particular type of hierarchical structure whereby the same phrase may be embedded within a phrase of the same type.

References

  • Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M.: Young children’s productivity with word order and verb morphology. Dev. Psychol. 33, 952–965 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albright, A., Hayes, B.: Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: a computational/experimental study. Cognition 90, 119–161 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, J., Seidenberg, M.S.: The emergence of grammaticality in connectionist networks. In: MacWhinney, B. (ed.) Emergentist Approaches to Language: Proceedings of the 28th Carnegie Symposium on Cognition, pp. 115–151. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., Pine, J.M., Rowland, C.F., Young, C.R.: The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children’s and adults’ graded judgments of argument-structure overgeneralization errors. Cognition 106, 87–129 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R.N., Jusczyk, P., Pisoni, D.B.: Speech and auditory processing during infancy: constraints on and precursors to language. In: Kuhn, D., Siegler, R. (eds.) Handbook of Child Psychology: Cognition, Perception, and Language, pp. 147–254. Wiley, New York (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, C.L.: Syntactic theory and the projection problem. Linguist. Inquiry 10, 533–581 (1979)

    Google Scholar 

  • Berko, J.: The child’s learning of english morphology. Word 14, 150–177 (1958)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M.: The ‘no negative evidence’ problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar? In: Hawkins, J.A. (ed.) Explaining Language Universals, pp. 73–101. Basil Blackwell, New York (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S.: On Two Types of Models of the Internalization of Grammars. In: Slobin, D.I. (ed.) The Ontogenesis of Grammar: A Theoretical Symposium, pp. 153–186. Academic Press, New York (1971)

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S., Brooks, P.J.: Verb argument strucure and the problem of avoiding an overgeneral grammar. In: Tomasello, M., Merriman, W.E. (eds.) Beyond Names for Things: Young Children’s Acquisition of Verbs, pp. 352–376. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, P.J., Tomasello, M.: Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Dev. Psychol. 35, 29–44 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., Hanlon, C.: Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In: Hayes, J.R. (ed.) Cognition and the Development of Language, pp. 11–53. Wiley, New York (1970)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, B. D. & Miikkulainen, R. (2001). From word stream to gestalt: A direct semantic parse for complex sentences. Technical Report TR-AI98-274, Department of Computer Sciences, The University of Texas at Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J.L.: Morphology: A Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form. John Benjamins, Amsterdam (1985)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J.L.: Diachronic and typological properties of morphology and their implications for representation. In: Feldman, L. (ed.) Morphological Aspects of Language Processing, pp. 225–246. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J.L., Moder, C.L.: Morphological classes as natural categories. Language 59, 251–270 (1983)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J.L., Slobin, D.I.: Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58, 265–289 (1982)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F.: Symbolically speaking: a connectionist model of sentence production. Cogn. Sci. 26, 609–651 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F.: Learning to order words: a connectionist model of heavy NP shift and accessibility effects in Japanese and English. J. Mem. Lang. 61, 374–397 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F., Dell, G.S., Bock, K.: Becoming syntactic. Psychol. Rev. 113, 234–272 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N., Tenenbaum, J.B., Yuille, A.: Probabilistic models of cognition: conceptual foundations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 287–291 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Syntactic Structures. Mouton, The Hague (1957)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge (1965)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Language and Mind. Harcourt, Brace and World, New York (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: The Minimalist Program. MIT Press, Cambridge (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Halle, M.: The Sound Pattern of English. Harper and Row, New York (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M.H., Chater, N.: Toward a connectionist model of recursion in human linguistic performance. Cogn. Sci. 23, 157–205 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M.H., Chater, N.: Language as shaped by the brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 489–509 (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, K.W.: A stochastic parts program and noun phrase parser for unrestricted text. In Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Applied Natural Language Processing, (pp. 136–143). ACL (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S., Pietroski, P.: Is generative grammar deceptively simple or simply deceptive? Lingua 116, 64–68 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W.: Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic Theory in Typological Perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Daugherty, K., Seidenberg, M.S.: Rules or Connections? The Past Tense Re-Visited. Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 259–264. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1992)

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLancey, S.: Adpositions as a Non-Universal Category. In: Frajzyngier, Z., Hodges, L., Rood, D.S. (eds.) Linguistic Diversity and Language Theories, pp. 185–202. John Benjamins, Amsterdam (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dowman, M.: Addressing the learnability of verb subcategorizations with Bayesian inference. In: Gleitman, L.R., Joshi, A.K. (eds.) Proceedings of the Twenty-Second Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J.L.: Finding structure in time. Cogn. Sci. 14, 179–211 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J.L.: Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small. Cognition 48, 71–99 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J.L., Bates, E.A., Johnson, M.H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., Plunkett, K.: Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. MIT Press, Cambridge (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, N., Levinson, S.C.: The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 429–492 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Everett, D.: Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Curr. Anthropol. 46, 621–646 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finch, S., Chater, N.: Distributional bootstrapping: from word class to proto-sentence. In: Eiselt, A.R.K. (ed.) Proceedings of the 16th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 301–306. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1994)

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C.: The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: a reply to Tomasello (2000). Cognition 82, 259–278 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garnsey, S.M., Pearlmutter, N.J., Meyers, E., Lotocky, M.A.: The contribution of verb bias to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. J. Mem. Lang. 37, 58–93 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., Eisengart, J.: Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychol. Sci. 17, 648–691 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L.R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A., Trueswell, J.C.: Hard words. Lang. Learn. Dev. 1(1), 23–64 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A.E.: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A.E.: Constructions At Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2005)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S.: The Resilience of Language: What Gesture Creation in Deaf Children Can Tell Us About How All Children Learn Language. Psychology Press, New York (2003)

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S.: What language creation in the manual modality tells us about the foundations of language. Linguist. Rev. 22, 199–225 (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonnerman, L.M., Seidenberg, M.S., Andersen, E.S.: Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 323–345 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J.H. (ed.): Universals of Language. MIT Press, Cambridge (1963)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Goldberg, R., Wilson, R.: The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language 65, 203–255 (1989)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., Mohanan, K.P.: Segmental phonology of modern English. Linguist. Inquiry 16, 57–116 (1985)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., Elman, J.L., Daugherty, K.G.: Default generalization in connectionist networks. Lang. Cogn. Process. 10, 601–630 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M.L., Ford, M., Marslen-Wilson, W.D.: Ambiguity and Frequency Effects in Regular Verb Inflection. In: Bybee, J., VHopper, P. (eds.) Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure, vol. 45, pp. 181–200. of Typographical Studies in LanguageJohn Benjamins, Amsterdam (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., Risely, J.: Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young Children. Brookes Publishing Co, Baltimore (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M.D., Chomsky, N., Fitch, W.T.: The faculty of language: what is it, who has it and how did it evolve? Science 298, 1569–1579 (2002)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J.B., Baayen, R.H.: Shifting paradigms: gradient structure in morphology. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 342–348 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson Kam, C.L., Newport, E.L.: Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages. Cogn. Psychol. 59, 30–66 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R.: Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, R.A.: What determines visual cue reliability? Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 345–350 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M.F., Seidenberg, M.S.: Impairments in verb morphology following brain injury: a connectionist model. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 96, 7592–7597 (1999)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M.F., Seidenberg, M.S.: Imaging the past: neural activation in frontal and temporal regions during regular and irregular past tense processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 282–296 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J.S., Newport, E.L.: Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of english as a second language. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 60–99 (1989)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kam, X.-N., Stoyneshka, I., Tornyova, L., Fodor, J., Sakas, W.: Bigrams and the richness of the stimulus. Cogn. Sci. 32, 771–787 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G.: Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1987)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Langlacker, R.W.: Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Volume 1, Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford University Press, Stanford (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J.D., Elman, J.L.: Learnability and the statistical structure of language: poverty of stimulus arguments revisited. In: Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, C., Thompson, S.A.: Coverbs in Mandarin Chinese: verbs or prepositions? J. Chin. Linguist. 2, 257–278 (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B.: The CHILDES project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. 3rd edn, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, C.D., Schutze, H.: Foundations of Statistical Natural Language Processing. MIT Press, Cambridge (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G.F., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., Pinker, S.: German inflection: the exception that proves the rule. Cogn. Psychol. 29, 189–256 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Tyler, L.: Morphology, language and the brain: the decompositional substrate for language comprehension. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 362, 823–836 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., Bybee, J.: Gradience of gradience: a reply to Jackendoff. Linguist. Rev. 24, 437–455 (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., Patterson, K.: Differentiation and integration in human language: a reply to Marslen-Wilson and Tyler. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 63–64 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miikkulainen, R.: Subsymbolic case-role analysis of sentences with embedded clauses. Cogn. Sci. 20, 47–73 (1996)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, T., Newport, E.L., Bever, T.: The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children. Cogn. Sci. 26, 393–424 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, T.H.: Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition 90, 91–117 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nevins, I., Pesetsky, D., Rodrigues, C.: Pirahã exceptionality: a reassessment. Language 85, 355–404 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E.L.: Maturational constraints on language learning. Cogn. Sci. 14, 11–28 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E.L., Aslin, R.: Innately constrained learning: blending old and new approaches to language acquisition. In: Howell, S.C., Fish, S.A., Keith-Lucas, T. (eds.) Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Cascadilla Press, Somerville (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E.L., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L.: Mother, I’d rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In: Snow, C., Ferguson, C.A. (eds.) Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1977)

    Google Scholar 

  • Onnis, L., Roberts, M., Chater, N.: Simplicity: A Cure for Overregularizations in Language Acquisition? Proceedings of the 24th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 720–725. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Mahwah (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfors, A., Tenenbaum, J.B., Wonnacott, E.: Variability, negative evidence, and the acquisition of verb argument constructions. J. Child Lang. 37, 607–642 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S.: Language Learnability and Language Development. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1984)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S.: The bootstrapping problem in language acquisition. In: MacWhinney, B. (ed.) Mechanisms of Language Acquisition. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S.: Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure. Learning, development and conceptual change. MIT Press, Cambridge (1989)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S.: Words and Rules. Basic Books, New York (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., Ullman, M.: The past and future of the past tense. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 456–463 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D.C., Gonnerman, L.M.: Are non-semantic morphological effects incompatible with a distributed connectionist approach to lexical processing? Lang. Cogn. Process. 15, 445–485 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., Marchman, V.: U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: implications for child language acquisition. Cognition 38, 43–102 (1991)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., Nakisa, R.C.: A connectionist model of the Arabic plural system. Lang. Cogn. Process. 12, 807–836 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, J.B.: Recursive auto-associative memory: Devising compositional distributed representations. Proceedings of the 10th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 33–39. Erlbaum, Hillsdale (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Post, B., Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Randall, B., Tyler, L.K.: The processing of English regular inflections: phonological cues to morphological structure. Cognition 109, 1–17 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S., Pinker, S.: Generalizations of regular and irregular morphology. Lang. Cogn. Process. 8, 1–56 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S., Pinker, S., Snyder, W.: Some evidence that irregular forms are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule generated. Paper presented at the Psychonomic Society meeting (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, A.: Transformational Grammar: A First Course. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1988)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reali, F., Christiansen, M.H.: Uncovering the richness of the stimulus: structure dependence and indirect statistical evidence. Cogn. Sci. 29, 1007–1028 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohde, D.L.T., Plaut, D.C.: Less is less in language acquisition. In: Quinlan, P. (ed.) Connectionist Modeling of Cognitive Development, pp. 189–231. Psychology Press, Hove (2003)

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D.A., Kenny, S., Derr, M.A.: Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 9, 86–102 (1983)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D., McClelland, J.: On Learning the past tenses of English verbs. In: McClelland, J., Rumelhart, D.: The PDP Research Group (eds.) Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, vol. 2, MIT Press, Cambridge (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N., Newport, E.L.: Statistical Learning by 8-month-old Infants. Science 274, 1926–1928 (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder, R., de Jong, N., Krott, A., Baayen, H.: Rules and rote: beyond the linguistic either-or fallacy. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 1038–1039 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S.: Language acquisition and use: learning and applying probabilistic constraints. Science 275, 1599–1604 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S., Pettito, L.A.: Signing behavior in apes: a critical review. Cognition 7, 177–215 (1979)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singleton, J.L., Newport, E.L.: When learners surpass their models: the acquisition of American sign language from inconsistent input. Cogn. Psychol. 49, 370–407 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J.C.: The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cogn. Psychol. 49, 238–299 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • St. John, M.F., McClelland, J.L.,: Learning and applying contextual constraints in sentence comprehension. Artif. Intell. 46, 217–257 (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Steedman, M.: Connectionist and symbolic representations of language. In: Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, Macmillan (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H.S., Petitto, L.A., Sanders, R.J., Bever, T.: Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206, 891–902 (1979)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Theakston, A.L.: The role of entrenchment in constraining children’s verb argument structure overgeneralisations: a grammaticality judgment study. Cogn. Dev. 19, 15–34 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M.: Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74, 209–253 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., Moll, H.: Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–735 (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Trueswell, J.C., Kim, A.E.: How to prune a garden-path by nipping it in the bud: Fast-priming of verb argument structures. J. Mem. Lang. 39, 102–123 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trueswell, J.C., Tanenhaus, M.K., Kello, C.: Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 528–553 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka, A.: The Semantics of Grammar. John Benjamins, Amsterdam (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wonnacott, E., Newport, E.L., Tanenhaus, M.K.: Acquiring and processing verb argument structure: distributional learning in a miniature language. Cogn. Psychol. 56, 165–209 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., Pinker, S.: Weird past tense forms. J. Child Lang. 22, 531–556 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to the following people for helpful discussions and/or comments on earlier drafts of the chapter: Adele Goldberg, Franklin Chang, Joanne Taylor, Jennifer Thomson and Edward Longhurst.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth Wonnacott .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wonnacott, E. (2013). Learning: Statistical Mechanisms in Language Acquisition. In: Binder, PM., Smith, K. (eds) The Language Phenomenon. The Frontiers Collection. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-36086-2_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-36086-2_4

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-36085-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-36086-2

  • eBook Packages: Physics and AstronomyPhysics and Astronomy (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics