Skip to main content

Operationalization and Data Analysis Methods

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Design of Incentive Systems

Part of the book series: Contributions to Management Science ((MANAGEMENT SC.))

  • 2027 Accesses

Abstract

Having introduced the expectations of this research in the previous chapter, this chapter deals with how the expectations are tested. First, it is presented how the individual attributes which are used in the hypotheses are operationalized and measured. This is followed by introducing the analysis methods for analyzing the hypotheses.

Measure what you can measure and make measureable what you cannot measure.

Galileo Galilei (1564 Pisa – 1642 Florence)

Cited in Gaarder (1996), p. 169

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Cf. Wright (2003), p. 133.

  2. 2.

    The term inventory is regularly used in psychology in order to describe a set of questions, which together form a hypothetical construct.

  3. 3.

    It shall be noted that the measures used not inevitably lead to the targeted individual attributes. In literature measurements are often said to lead to hypothetical constructs, which are supposed to approximate the individual attribute, which shall be measured. See e.g. Heckhausen (1980), pp. ff. for a discussion concerning hypothetical constructs.

  4. 4.

    Cf. Mauldin (2003), p. 29; GRE is a standardized test created and administered by the Educational Testing Service. Refer to http://www.ets.org/ for further information.

  5. 5.

    Cf. Dohmen and Falk (2011).

  6. 6.

    Cf. Baiman and Lewis (1989), p. 9.

  7. 7.

    Cf. Waller and Chow (1985), p. 462.

  8. 8.

    Cf. McClelland et al. (1953); Schmalt (1976), p. 18.

  9. 9.

    Cf. Schmalt and Sokolowski (2000), p. 115.

  10. 10.

    Cf. Biernat (1989), p. 69; Brunstein and Heckhausen (2006), p. 154.

  11. 11.

    Cf. Schmalt (1976).

  12. 12.

    Cf. Stein (1990), p. 235.

  13. 13.

    Refer to Brunstein and Heckhausen (2006), pp. 145ff. for a review of measuring the motive to achieve.

  14. 14.

    Cf. Tent (1963); Mikula et al. (1976); Dahme et al. (1993).

  15. 15.

    Cf. Mehrabian (1968); Mehrabian (1969); Brunstein and Heckhausen (2006), pp. 154ff.

  16. 16.

    Cf. Mikula et al. (1976), pp. 90ff.

  17. 17.

    Cf. Matiakse and Stein (1992), pp. 247 f.

  18. 18.

    Cf. Mikula et al. (1976), p. 91; Matiakse and Stein (1992), p. 246; Cronbach’s alpha, calculated for the 134 participants who report a German mother tongue, results in 0.36. Cronbach’s alpha is equivalent to the Kuder-Richardson 20 procedure, because of the binary response format.

  19. 19.

    If Cronbach’s alpha is only calculated with these six items, the value is 0.37. Refer to Sect. 3.2.4.2 for details concerning the items-to-total-score correlation test.

  20. 20.

    Cf. Matiakse and Stein (1992), pp. 246ff.

  21. 21.

    Cf. Cacioppo and Petty (1982); Cacioppo et al. (1984); Bless et al. (1994).

  22. 22.

    Please refer to the appendix for the single items.

  23. 23.

    Cf. Bless et al. (1994), pp. 149f.

  24. 24.

    Cf. Gulgoz (2001), p. 103; Cacioppo et al. (1984) and Bless et al. (1994) do not state mean values.

  25. 25.

    Cf. Cacioppo et al. (1984), p. 306; Bless et al. (1994), p. 149; Gulgoz (2001), p. 102.

  26. 26.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), pp. 57ff.; The single factors forming the hypothetical construct also appear independently from each other in literature (Cf. e.g. Scott Jr. (1967); Farh et al. (1991)).

  27. 27.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), p. 58.

  28. 28.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), pp. supplemental material.

  29. 29.

    A different approach to measure interest in a task or task attractiveness is used by Scott Jr. (1967) or Farh et al. (1991). They do not use complete questions but use opposite ratings such as attractive versus unattractive as an instrument. Since Rheinberg et al. (2001), p. 58 provide an integration of the factor interest into the motivation concept, their approach is pursued for the factor interest as well as for the other factors in order to measure a current motivation construct.

  30. 30.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), pp. 58f.

  31. 31.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), pp. supplemental material.

  32. 32.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), pp. supplemental material.

  33. 33.

    In the underlying research probability of success is also captured by (L.1/ SKILL) ∙ L.2, which is denoted L.PROBABILITYOFSUCCESS.

  34. 34.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), p. 58.

  35. 35.

    Cf. Rheinberg et al. (2001), p. 59.

  36. 36.

    Since the factor probability of success merely consists of one question, Cronbach’s alpha cannot be calculated.

  37. 37.

    Cf. Krampen (1979), p. 579; Refer to Krampen (1982), p. 115 for an extensive survey of German and English locus of control inventories.

  38. 38.

    Cf. Levenson (1974).

  39. 39.

    Cf. Rotter (1966), p. 1; Levenson (1974); Logsdon et al. (1978), p. 538; Krampen (1982), p. 108.

  40. 40.

    Cf. Logsdon et al. (1978), p. 538.

  41. 41.

    Cf. Krampen (1979), p. 579.

  42. 42.

    Cf. Krampen (1979), p. 581.

  43. 43.

    Cf. Krampen (1979), p. 581.

  44. 44.

    Cf. Krampen (1979), p. 579.

  45. 45.

    Cf. MacCrimmon and Wehrung (1985b), p. 10.

  46. 46.

    Cf. Botella et al. (2008), pp. 530 f.; The different categories are not mutually exclusive. Refer to Skeel et al. (2007) for a study investigating predictions of risky behavior with a multiple-factor personality inventory (NEO-inventory).

  47. 47.

    Cf. Botella et al. (2008), pp. 530 f.; Participants’ honest contribution is not only key to the success of questionnaire-based risk measures, but also to the success of the whole study. For further comments refer to Sect. 3.2.4.3.

  48. 48.

    MacCrimmon and Wehrung (1985b), p. 22.

  49. 49.

    Cf. MacCrimmon and Wehrung (1985b), p. 24.

  50. 50.

    Refer to Harrison et al. (2005), pp. 1394ff. for a review on risk measurements.

  51. 51.

    Cf. Hyatt and Taylor (2008), pp. supplemental material.

  52. 52.

    Cf. Botella et al. (2008), p. 531.

  53. 53.

    Cf. Shields and Waller (1988), pp. 585–586; Cadsby et al. (2007).

  54. 54.

    Cf. Shields and Waller (1988), p. 590.

  55. 55.

    Cf. Shields and Waller (1988), p. 585.

  56. 56.

    Cf. Holt and Laury (2002).

  57. 57.

    Cf. Holt and Laury (2002); Dohmen and Falk (2006), p. 10; Cadsby et al. (2007), p. 390.

  58. 58.

    Cf. Holt and Laury (2002), p. 1645.

  59. 59.

    Cf. Holt and Laury (2002), p. 1646.

  60. 60.

    Cf. Weber et al. (2002).

  61. 61.

    Cf. Weber et al. (2002); Johnson et al. (2004); Harrison et al. (2005), p. 1394; Blais and Weber (2006).

  62. 62.

    Cf. Sitkin and Weingart (1995), p. 1592; Williams et al. (2008), p. 66.

  63. 63.

    Cf. Betsch (2004), p. 183.

  64. 64.

    Cf. Schunk and Betsch (2006), p. 399.

  65. 65.

    Cf. Betsch (2004), p. 183; Schunk and Betsch (2006), p. 392.

  66. 66.

    Cf. Jaccard (2005a); Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 8; Field (2007), pp. 309ff.

  67. 67.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 10; See also Allison (2004), pp. 20ff. for a discussion of the advantages of multiple regression in the context of experiments.

  68. 68.

    Cf. Field (2009), pp. 395ff.

  69. 69.

    Cf. Hardy (2007), p. 1; Cohen (2009), pp. 1f.

  70. 70.

    There is a discussion in literature whether data from rating scales can be treated as continuous data. The general understanding is that rating scales with enough steps approximate the metric quality of true continuous scales. In this text Srinivasan and Basu (1989), p. 226’s opinion is followed that five levels provide good metric quality. Thus, the five level scales are supposed to result in continuous measures in the study.

  71. 71.

    Cf. Wendorf (2004), p. 47; Hardy (2007); Field (2007), pp. 309ff.; Refer to Wendorf (2004), p. 47 for a comparison of contrast procedures between the ANOVA and regression paradigm.

  72. 72.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 10; Cohen (2009), pp. 4f.; Refer to Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 10ff. for a discussion of different effect size indices with a focus to interaction effects.

  73. 73.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 10; Cohen (2009), p. 5.

  74. 74.

    Cf. Anderson (1986), pp. 188f.; Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 15, 65ff.

  75. 75.

    Cohen (2009), p. 151, comparing regression with correlational analysis, state that regression coefficients often lead to the most informative results and answers. Refer to Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1175 for details concerning deficiencies, or to Cohen (2009), pp. 151–192 for a detailed methodological comparison of regression and correlational analysis.

  76. 76.

    Refer to Fahrmeir et al. (2007), pp. 55ff. or Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 50ff. for an overview of different functional relationships.

  77. 77.

    Cf. Henderson (1998); The SPSS package used offers both analysis techniques. It is important to note that regression, if based on the general linear model, and ANOVA, if also based on the general linear model, are conceptually the same. In the SPSS package used, the general linear model is used for both paradigms (Cf. Field (2007), p. 311; Cohen (2009), p. 5).

  78. 78.

    Cf. Rack and Christophersen (2007), p. 19.

  79. 79.

    Cf. Evans and Rooney (2008), pp. 194f.

  80. 80.

    Cf. Irwin and McClelland (2003), p. 366; Ravichandran and Fitzmaurice (2008), p. 610.

  81. 81.

    Cf. Irwin and McClelland (2003), p. 366.

  82. 82.

    Cf. Irwin and McClelland (2003), pp. 366ff.; Ravichandran and Fitzmaurice (2008), p. 610.

  83. 83.

    Cf. Irwin and McClelland (2003), p. 369.

  84. 84.

    Cf. Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1176; Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 49.

  85. 85.

    Cf. Irwin and McClelland (2003), p. 371.

  86. 86.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 49.

  87. 87.

    Ordinary least squares is the method used for estimating the linear regression parameters.

  88. 88.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 10f.

  89. 89.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 33; For comparison reason, additionally, standardized coefficients are given in the relevant tables.

  90. 90.

    Cf. Hardy (2007), pp. 19ff.

  91. 91.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 25f.

  92. 92.

    Cf. Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1176.

  93. 93.

    Cf. Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1174.

  94. 94.

    Cf. Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), p. 86; Fahrmeir et al. (2007), pp. 19 ff., 25; Fahrmeir et al. (2007), pp. 25 ff. show exemplarily that by other transformations of predictor variables, also non-linear relationships can be implemented into the GLM.

  95. 95.

    Cf. Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1174.

  96. 96.

    Cf. Janssen and Laatz (2007), pp. 426f.

  97. 97.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 74,116; Janssen and Laatz (2007), pp. 421f.; Field (2007), pp. 151ff.

  98. 98.

    SPSS generally states two-tailed significance values. However, as directed hypotheses are formulated, significance values based on one-tailed tests are also appropriate as suggested for example in Allison (2004), p. 42, Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 76f. or Janssen and Laatz (2007), p. 426. Still, as it is standard practice in linear regression to report two-tailed tests, significance levels based on two-tailed tests are reported in the underlying experiment. Thus, the significance levels can be regarded as being rather conservative.

  99. 99.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 16; Hardy (2007), pp. 48f.

  100. 100.

    For instance, Cadsby et al. (2007), p. 397 or Mauldin (2003), p. 35 only analyze product term coefficients for investigating interaction effects.

  101. 101.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), p. 24; Menard (2000), p. 54.

  102. 102.

    Cf. Anderson (1986), p. 191; Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 18, 21.

  103. 103.

    Cf. Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1177.

  104. 104.

    Cf. Baron and Kenny (1986), p. 1177.

  105. 105.

    Cf. Sitkin and Weingart (1995), pp. 1582f.

  106. 106.

    Cf. Field (2007), pp. 111f. Albeit this guideline actually refers to effect sizes of correlation coefficients, it can be taken as guideline in various effect size measures as e.g. in Field (2007), pp. 172, 357.

  107. 107.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 24f.

  108. 108.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 17f.

  109. 109.

    In linear regression analysis SPSS merely shows differences between unadjusted \( {{\mathrm{ R}}^2} \) s as well.

  110. 110.

    Cf. Field (2007), pp. 171f.

  111. 111.

    As the number of cases and the number of predictor variables are also reported, any adjustments to \( {{\mathrm{ R}}^2} \) can be still performed without needing further information. It shall be also noted that adjustments to \( {{\mathrm{ R}}^2} \) based on Wherry’s equation within the underlying analyses are quantitatively small and do not change the analysis qualitatively.

  112. 112.

    Cf. Schroeder et al. (2005), pp. 73ff.; Berry (2006), pp. 1ff.; Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), pp. 85ff., 233ff.; Field (2007), pp. 169ff.

  113. 113.

    Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 94 note that regression analysis is relatively robust against small violations of its assumptions.

  114. 114.

    Cf. Schroeder et al. (2005), pp. 24ff.; Field (2007), p. 186.

  115. 115.

    Cf. Jaccard et al. (1997), pp. 30ff.

  116. 116.

    Cf. Field (2007), p. 175.

  117. 117.

    Cf. Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), p. 238; Field (2007), p. 196.

  118. 118.

    Cf. Durbin and Watson (1951), pp. 159ff.; Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), p. 201; Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 89.

  119. 119.

    Cf. Schroeder et al. (2005), p. 67; Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 102.

  120. 120.

    Cf. Goldfeld and Quandt (1965); Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 89; Hackl (2008), pp. 178f.; Refer to Eckey et al. (2005) for an applied introduction to the application of the Goldfeld-Quandt, White and Breusch-Pagan test within SPSS.

  121. 121.

    Cf. Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), pp. 90ff.; Field (2007), pp. 202ff.

  122. 122.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 92f.

  123. 123.

    Cf. Cook (1977); Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), pp. 88, 103ff.; Field (2007), pp. 165ff., 199ff.

  124. 124.

    Cf. Chatterjee and Hadi (2006), pp. 88, 103f.

  125. 125.

    Cf. Field (2007), p. 165; In the findings section Cook’s distance values higher than 1.25 are further investigated.

  126. 126.

    Cf. Menard (2000), pp. 1–16.

  127. 127.

    Cf. Norušis (2010), p. 69.

  128. 128.

    Cf. Bühl (2006), p. 111.

  129. 129.

    Cf. Bühl (2006), pp. 353f., 372ff.; Multinomial regression (regression with dependent variables having more than two categories) has also been called polytomous or polychotomous (Cf. Menard (2000), p. 80).

  130. 130.

    When the magnitude of incentive variable is used as predictor, the linear ordinary least squares regression analysis can be used independently from the variable’s classification being ordinal or nominal.

  131. 131.

    Cf. Janssen and Laatz (2007), p. 459.

  132. 132.

    For a discussion about how to treat ordinal dependent variables refer to Menard (2000), pp. 86ff.

  133. 133.

    Refer to Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 425ff. or Menard (2000) for a detailed introduction to logistic regression and Jaccard (2005b) for an introduction to logistic regression with a focus on interactions.

  134. 134.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 428.

  135. 135.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 471f.

  136. 136.

    Cf. Liao (2006), p. 7.

  137. 137.

    Cf. Menard (2000), pp. 38ff.; Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 460f.

  138. 138.

    Cf. Field (2007), p. 224; see Hosmer and Lemeshow (2000), p. 35 for details concerning the derivation of logistic regression coefficients’ standard errors.

  139. 139.

    Differences of Nagelkerke’s pseudo R2 values are evaluated qualitatively, as no test is introduced in the underlying literature.

  140. 140.

    Cf. Field (2007), p. 223.

  141. 141.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), pp. 449, 456.

  142. 142.

    Cf. Menard (2000), p. 32; In this context, classification relates to a model’s prediction for an element belonging to one or the other category. For instance, by means of classification tables, the number of correctly predicted categories can be compared to the falsely predicted categories. For further methods to gauge the classification accuracy of logistic regression models refer to Menard (2000), pp. 28ff. In general goodness-of-fit and accuracy of classification measures produce consistent results (Menard (2000), p. 32).

  143. 143.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 440; Liao (2006), p. 18.

  144. 144.

    Refer to Dohmen and Falk (2011) for an example for the use of marginal effect sizes in probit models.

  145. 145.

    Refer to Liao (2006), pp. 6ff., 13ff. for an in-depth discussion of parameter estimates’ interpretation possibilities.

  146. 146.

    Cf. Backhaus et al. (2006), p. 480.

  147. 147.

    Refer to Sect. 3.2.3 for a more general discussion on sample size.

  148. 148.

    Refer to Hardy (2007), pp. 9ff. or Fahrmeir et al. (2007), pp. 80ff. for further information about dummy coding.

References

  • Allison PD (2004) Multiple regression: a primer. Pine Forge Press, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson CH (1986) Hierarchical moderated regression analysis: a useful tool for retail management decisions. J Retailing 62(2):186–203

    Google Scholar 

  • Backhaus K, Erichson B, Plinke W, Weiber R (2006) Multivariate analysemethoden: eine anwendungsorientierte einführung, 11th edn. Heidelberg, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Baiman S, Lewis BL (1989) An experiment testing the behavioral equivalence of strategically equivalent employment contracts. J Accounting Res 27(1):1–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51(6):1173–1182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry WD (2006) Understanding regression assumptions. Sage, Newbury Park

    Google Scholar 

  • Betsch C (2004) Präferenz für Intuition und Deliberation (PID): Inventar zur Erfassung von affekt- und kognitionsbasiertem Entscheiden [Preference for Intuition and Deliberation (PID): An Inventory for Assessing Affect- and Cognition-Based Decision-Making]. in: Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 25(4), 179–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Biernat M (1989) Motives and values to achieve: different constructs with different effects. J Pers 57(1):69–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blais A-R, Weber EU (2006) A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgm Decis Making 1(1):33–47

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless H, Wänke M, Bohner G, Fellhauer R, Schwarz N (1994) Need for Cognition: Eine Skala zur Erfassung von Freude und Engagement bei Denkaufgaben. in: Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 25, 147–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Botella J, Narváez M, Martínez-Molina A, Rubio VJ, Santacreu J (2008) A dilemmas task for eliciting risk propensity. Psychol Rec 58(4):528–545

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein J, Heckhausen H (2006) Leistungsmotivation. In: Heckhausen J, Heckhausen H (eds) Motivation und Handeln, 3rd edn. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 144–192

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühl A (2006) SPSS 14: einführung in die moderne datenanalyse, 10th edn. Pearson Studium, München

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo JT, Petty RE (1982) The need for cognition. J Pers Soc Psychol 42:116–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Kao CF (1984) The efficient assessment of need for cognition. J Pers Assess 48(3):306–307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cadsby CB, Song F, Tapon F (2007) Sorting and incentive effects of pay for performance: an experimental investigation. Acad Manage J 50(2):387–405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee S, Hadi AS (2006) Regression analysis by example, 4th edn. Wiley-Interscience, Hoboken

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (2009) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences, 3rd edn. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook RD (1977) Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics 19:15–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahme G, Jungnickel D, Rathje H (1993) Güteeigenschaften der Achievement Motivation Scale (AMS) von Gjesme und Nygard (1970) in der deutschen Übersetzung von Göttert und Kuhl: Vergleich der Kennwerte norwegischer und deutscher Stichproben. Diagnostica 39:257–270

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen T, Falk A (2006) Performance pay and multi-dimensional sorting: productivity, preferences and gender. Working paper: IZA discussion paper, Bonn

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen T, Falk A (2011) Performance pay and multi-dimensional sorting: productivity, preferences and gender. Am Econ Rev 101(2):556–590

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durbin J, Watson GS (1951) Testing for serial correlation in least squares regression. Biometrika 38(1/2):159–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckey H.-f, Kosfeld R, Türck M (eds) (2005) Ökonometrische Eingleichungsmodelle mit SPSS: Eine Einführung. Working paper: Universität Kassel

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans AN, Rooney BJ (2008) Methods in psychological research. Sage, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahrmeir L, Kneib T, Lang S (2007) Regression: modelle, methoden und anwendungen. Springer, Berlin\Heidelberg

    Google Scholar 

  • Farh J-L, Griffeth RW, Balkin DB (1991) Effects of choice of pay plans on satisfaction, goal setting, and performance. J Organ Behav 12:55–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field AP (2007) Discovering statistics using SPSS, 2nd edn. Sage, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Field A (2009) Discovering statistics using SPSS, 3rd edn. Sage, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldfeld SM, Quandt RE (1965) Some tests for homoscedasticity. J Am Stat Assoc 60(310):539–547

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gulgoz S (2001) Need for cognition and cognitive performance from a cross-cultural perspective: examples of academic success and solving anagrams. J Psychol 135(1):100–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackl P (2008) Einführung in die Ökonometrie. München

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy MA (2007) Regression with dummy variables. Sage, Newbury Park

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison JD, Young JM, Butow P, Salkeld G, Solomon MJ (2005) Is it worth the risk? a systematic review of instruments that measure risk propensity for use in the health setting. Soc Sci Med 60(6):1385–1396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen H (1980) Motivation und handeln. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson C (ed) (1998) The SPSS GLM procedure, or what happened to ANOVA. Working paper, University of north Texas, Research and statistical support services

    Google Scholar 

  • Holt CA, Laury SK (2002) Risk aversion and incentive effects. Am Econ Rev 92(5):1644–4655

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S (2000) Applied logistic regression, 2nd edn. Wiley, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hyatt TA, Taylor MH (2008) The effects of incomplete personal capability knowledge and overconfidence on employment contract selection. Behav Res Account 20(2):37–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin JR, McClelland GH (2003) Negative consequences of dichotomizing continuous predictor variables. J Market Res 40(3):366–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaccard J (2005a) Interaction effects in factorial analysis of variance. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaccard J (2005b) Interaction effects in logistic regression. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaccard J, Turrisi R, Wan CK (1997) Interaction effects in multiple regression, 9th edn. Sage, Newbury Park

    Google Scholar 

  • Janssen J, Laatz W (2007) Statistische Datenanalyse mit SPSS für Windows: Eine anwendungsorientierte Einführung in das Basissystem und das Modul Exakte Tests, 6. ed. Berlin, Heidelberg

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson JG, Wilke A, Weber EU (2004) A domain-specific scale measuring risk perception, expected benefits and perceived-risk attitude in German-speaking populations. Pol Psychol Bull 35(3):153–163

    Google Scholar 

  • Krampen G (1979) Differenzierungen des Konstruktes der Kontrollüberzeugungen. in: Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 26(4), pp. 573–595

    Google Scholar 

  • Krampen G (1982) Differentialpsychologie der Kontrollüberzeugungen: (‘Locus of control’). Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenson H (1974) Activism and powerful others: distinctions within the concept of internal-external control. J Pers Assess 38:377–383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liao TF (2006) Interpreting probability models: logit, probit, and other generalized linear models. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Logsdon SA, Bourgeois AE, Levenson H (1978) Locus of control, learned helplessness, and control of heart rate using biofeedback. J Pers Assess 42(5):538–544

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCrimmon KR, Wehrung DA (1985) A portfolio of risk measures. Theor Decis 19:1–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matiakse W, Stein FA (1992) Gütekontrolle zweier Leistungsmotivationstests. Diagnostica 38(3):242–248

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauldin EG (2003) An experimental examination of information technology and compensation structure complementarities in an expert system context. J Inform Syst 17(1):19–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland DC, Atkinson JW, Clark RA, Lowell EL (1953) The achievement motive. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian A (1968) Male and female scales of the tendency to achieve. Educ Psychol Meas 28(2):493–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian A (1969) Measures of achieving tendency. Educ Psychol Meas 29(2):445–451

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menard S (2000) Applied logistic regression analysis. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Mikula G, Uray H, Schwinger T (1976) Die Entwicklung einer deutschen Fassung der Mehrabian Achievement Risk Preference Scale. Diagnostica 22:87–97

    Google Scholar 

  • Norušis MJ (2010) PASW Statistics 18 Advanced Statistical procedures. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack O, Christophersen T (2007) Experimente. In: Albers, Sönke; Klapper, Daniel; Konradt, Udo; Walter, Achim; Wolf, Joachim (ed, 2007): Methodik der empirischen Forschung. 2. ed. Wiesbaden, pp. 17–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravichandran C, Fitzmaurice GM (2008) To dichotomize or not to dichotomize? Nutrition 24(6):610–611

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg F, Vollmeyer R, Burns BD (2001) FAM: Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung aktueller Motivation in Lern- und Leistungssituationen. Diagnostica 47(2):57–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter JB (1966) Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcment. Psychol Monogr 80:1–609

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmalt H-D (1976) Die Messung des Leistungsmotivs. Verlag für Psychologie Hogrefe, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmalt H-D, Sokolowski K (2000) Zum gegenwärtigen Stand der Motivdiagnostik. Diagnostica 46(3):115–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder LD, Sjoquist DL, Stephan PE (2005) Understanding regression analysis: an introductory guide. Sage, Newbury Park, Calif

    Google Scholar 

  • Schunk D, Betsch C (2006) Explaining heterogeneity in utility functions by individual differences in decision modes. J Econ Psychol 27(3):386–401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott WE Jr (1967) The development of semantic differential scales as measures of “morale”. Pers Psychol 20(2):179–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shields MD, Waller WS (1988) A behavioral study of accounting variables in performance – incentive contracts. Account Organ Soc 13(6):581–594

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sitkin SB, Weingart LR (1995) Determinants of risky decision-making behavior: a test of the mediating role of risk perceptions and propensity. Acad Manage J 38(6):1573–1592

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skeel RL, Neudecker J, Pilarski C, Pytlak K (2007) The utility of personality variables and behaviorally-based measures in the prediction of risk-taking behavior. Pers Indiv Differ 43(1):203–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan V, Basu AK (1989) The metric quality of ordered categorical data. Market Sci 8(8):205–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein FA (1990) Betriebliche Entscheidungs-Situationen im Laborexperiment: Die Abbildung von Aufgaben- und Strukturmerkmalen als Validitätsbedingungen. Frankfurt am Main

    Google Scholar 

  • Tent L (1963) Untersuchung zur Erfassung des Verhältnisses von Anspannung und Leistung bei vorwiegend psychisch beanspruchenden Tätigkeiten. Arch Gesamte Psychol 115:105–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Waller WS, Chow CW (1985) The self-selection and effort effects of standard-based employment contracts: a framework and some empirical evidence. Account Rev 60(3):458–476

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber EU, Blais A-RE, Betz NE (2002) A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. J Behav Decis Making 15:263–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wendorf CA (2004) Primer on multiple regression coding: common forms and the additional case of repeated contrasts. Underst Stat 3(1):47–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams S, Zainuba M, Jackson R (2008) Determinants of managerial risk perceptions and intentions. J Manage Res 8(2):59–75

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright DB (2003) Making friends with your data: improving how statistics are conducted and reported. Br J Educ Psychol 73(1):123–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Fehrenbacher, D.D. (2013). Operationalization and Data Analysis Methods. In: Design of Incentive Systems. Contributions to Management Science. Physica, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-33599-0_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics