Skip to main content

Frontalhirninfarkte: Defizite der Theory of Mind und anderer Leistungen

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Theory of Mind

Zusammenfassung

Die Versorgung des Frontalhirns erfolgt aus der Halsschlagader, der Arteria carotis interna (ACI), die sich intrakraniell in die Arteria cerebri anterior (ACA) und Arteria cerebri media (ACM) aufteilt. Abb. 29.1 und Abb. 29.2 zeigen die arteriellen Versorgungsgebiete der Arterien.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Literatur

  • Abe K, Inokawa M, Kashiwagi A, Yanagihara T (1998) Amnesia after a discrete basal forebrain lesion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 65(1): 126–130

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Abell F, Happé F, Frith U (2000) Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn Dev 15(1): 1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abraham A, Rakoczy H, Werning M et al (2010) Matching mind to world and vice versa: functional dissociations between belief and desire mental state processing. Soc Neurosci 5(1): 1–18

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly I, Samson D, Chiavarino D, Humphreys G (2004) Frontal and temporo parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci 16(10): 1773–1784

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly I, Samson D, Chiavarino D et al (2007) Testing the domain-specificity of a theory of mind deficit in brain-injured patients: Evidence for consistent performance on non-verbal, »reality-unknown« false belief and false photograph tasks. Cognition 103: 300–321

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arboix AL, García-Eroles L, Sellares N et al. (2009) Infarction in the territory of the anterior cerebral artery: clinical study of 51 patients. BMC Neurology 9: 30

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aschersleben G, Hofer T, Jovanovic B (2008) The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Dev Sci 11: 862–868

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bamford J, Sandercock P, Dennis M, Burn J, Warlow C (1991) Classification and natural history of clinically identifiable subtypes of cerebral infarction. Lancet 337: 1521–1526

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara A (2004) The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn 55(1): 30–40

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benoit RG, Gilbert SJ, Volle E, Burgess P (2010) When I think about me and simulate you: medial rostral prefrontal cortex and self-referential processes, NeuroImage 50: 1340–1349

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berthier ML, Starkstein SE, Leiguarda R et al (1991) Transcortical aphasia. Importance of the nonspeech dominant hemisphere in language repetition. Brain 114(Pt 3): 1409–1427

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bird CM, Castelli F, Malik O et al (2004) The impact of extensive medial front lobe damage on »Theory of Mind« and cognition. Brain 127 (Pt4): 914–928

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bogousslavsky J, Regli F (1990) Anterior cerebral artery territory infarction in the Lausanne Stroke Registry. Clinical and etiologic patterns. Arch Neurol 47(2): 144–150

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Channon S, Rule A, Maudgil D et al (2007) Interpretation of mentalistic actions and sarcastic remarks: Effects of frontal and posterior lesions on mentalising. Neuropsychologia 45: 1725–1734

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings JL (1993) Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol 50(8): 873–880

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • David N, Bewernick BH, Cohen MX et al (2006) Neural representations of self versus other: visual-spatial perspective taking and agency in a virtual ball-tossing game. Cogn Neurosci 18(6): 898–910

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Della Sala S, Francescani A, Spinnler H (2002) Gait apraxia after bilateral supplementary motor area lesion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72(1): 77–85

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferstl EC, von Cramon DY (2002) What does the frontomedian cortex contribute to language processing: coherence or theory of mind? NeuroImage 17(3): 1599–1612

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fine C, Lumsden J, Blair RJ (2001) Dissociation between »theory of mind« and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain 124(Pt 2): 287–298

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith U, Frith CD (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358(1431): 459-473

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giroud M, Lemesle M, Madinier G et al (1997) Unilateral lenticular infarcts: radiological and clinical syndromes, aetiology, and prognosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 63(5): 611–615

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein K (1917) Die transkorikalen Aphasien. Fischer, Jena

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossi D, Trojano L, Chiacchio L et al (1991) Mixed transcortical aphasia: clinical features and neuroanatomical correlates. A possible role of the right hemisphere. Eur Neurol 31(4): 204–211

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98(7): 4259–4264

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Happé FG, Brownell H, Winner E (1999) Acquired »theory of mind« impairments following stroke. Cognition 70(3): 211–240

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F, Malhi GS, Checkley S (2001) Acquired mind-blindness following frontal lobe surgery? A single case study of impaired ‚theory of mind‘ in a patient treated with stereotactic anteriorcapsulotomy. Neuropsychologia 39(1): 83–90

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann F (1917) Beiträge zur Apraxielehre. Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol 21: 97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Havet-Thomassin V, Allain P, Etcharry-Bouyx F, Le Gall D (2006) What about theory of mind after severe brain injury? Brain Inj 20(1): 83–91

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Igliori G, Damasceno B (2006) Theory of Mind and the frontal lobes. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 64(2-A): 202–206

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalbe E, Schlegel M, Sack AT et al (2010) Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: a TMS study. Cortex 46(6): 769–780

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kang SY, Kim JS (2008) Anterior cerebral artery infarction: stroke mechanism and clinical-imaging study in 100 patients. Neurology 70(24): 2386–2393

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karnath HO, Fruhmann Berger M, Kuker W, Rorden C (2004) The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cerebr Cortex 14(10): 1164–1172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karnath H, Suchan J et al (2008) Pusher syndrome after ACA territory infarction. Eur J Neurol 15(8): 84–85

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim JS (2001) Involuntary movements after anterior cerebral artery territory infarction. Stroke 32(1): 258–261

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer JH, Reed BR, Mungas D et al (2002) Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischaemic vascular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72(2): 217–220

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kucharska-Pietura K, Phillips M, Gernand W, David A (2003) Perception of emotions from faces and voices following unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia 41: 1082–1090

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kumral E, Bayulkem G, Evyapan D, Yunten N (2002) Spectrum of anterior cerebral artery territory infarction: clinical and MRI findings. Eur J Neurol 9(6): 615–624

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lausberg H, Gottert R, Munssinger U et al (1999) Callosal disconnection syndrome in a left-handed patient due to infarction of the total length of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia 37(3): 253–265

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leistner S, Boegner F, Marx P, Koennecke HC (2001) Transtentorial herniation after unilateral infarction of the anterior cerebral artery. Stroke 32(3): 649–651

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leudar I, Costall A, Francis D (2004) Theory of mind. A critical assessment. Theory Psychol 14(5): 571-578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin I, McDonald S (2006) That can’t be right – What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment 7: 202–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martín-Rodríguez JF, Léon-Carrión J (2010) Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: a quantitative review. Neuropsychologia 48: 1181–1191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthes-von Cramon G, von Cramon Y (2000) Störungen exekutiver Funktionen. In: Sturm MHW, Wallesch C-W (Hrsg) Lehrbuch der klinischen Neuropsychologie. Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse, S 392–410

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx P (1977) Die Gefäßerkrankungen von Hirn und Rückenmark. Gustav Fischer, Stuttgart

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald S, Pearce S (1996) Clinical insights into pragmatic theory: frontal lobe deficits and sarcasm. Brain Language 53: 81–104

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald S, Flanagan S (2004) Social perception deficits after traumatic brain injury: interaction between emotion recognition, mentalizing ability, and social communication. Neuropsychology 18(3): 572–579

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mendez MF (2004) Aphemia-like syndrome from a right supplementary motor area lesion. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 106(4): 337–339

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milders M, Ietswaart M, Currie D, Crawford J (2006) Impairments in theory of mind shortly after traumatic brain injury and at 1-year follow-up. Neuropsychology 20(4): 400–408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell JP, Banaji MR, Macrae CN (2005) General and specific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about mental states. NeuroImage 28: 757–762

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell JP (2008) Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. Cereb Cortex 18(2): 262–271

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mohr JP (1973) Rapid amelioration of motor aphasia. Arch Neurol 28(2): 77–82

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perner J, Wimmer H (1985) »John thinks that Mary thinks that«. Attribution of second order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. J Exp Psychol 39(17): 437–471

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Ploner CJ et al (2003) Decisional role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behaviour. Brain 126(Pt 6): 1460–1473

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reed BR, Eberling JL, Mungas D et al (2004) Effects of white matter lesions and lacunes on cortical function. Arch Neurol 61(10): 1545–1550

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe AD, Bullock PR, Polkey CE, Morris RG (2001) »Theory of mind« impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain 124(Pt 3): 600–616

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Samson D, Apperly I, Chiavarino C, Humphreys G (2004) Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else´s belief. Nat Neurosci 7(5): 499–500

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe R, Xiao DK, Kovacs G et al (2004) A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42(11):1435–1446

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe R, Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43: 1391–1399

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Aharon-Peretz J (2005) The neuroanatomical basis of understanding sarcasm and its relationship to social cognition. Neuropsychology 19(3): 288–300

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tibi-Elhanany Y, Aharon-Peretz J (2006) The ventromedial prefrontal cortex is involved in affective but not cognitive theory of mind stories. Soc Neurosci 1(3–4): 149–166

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D (2009) Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132: 617–627

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw P, Lawrence EJ, Radbourne C et al (2004) The impact of early and late damage to the human amygdala on »theory of mind« reasoning. Brain 127(Pt 7): 1535–1548

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M, Varley R (2002) Neural systems involved in »theory of mind«. Nature Rev Neurosci 3(6): 463–471

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT (1998) Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci 10(5): 640–656

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Calder A et al (2003) Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 41(2): 209–220

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss DT, Gallup GG Jr, Alexander MP (2001) The frontal lobes are necessary for »theory of mind«. Brain 124(Pt 2): 279–286

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss DT, Anderson V (2004) The frontal lobes and theory of mind: developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research. Brain Cogn 55(1): 69–83

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surian L, Siegal M (2001) Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Language 78: 224–232

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ticini L, Klose U, Nägele T Karnath H (2009) Perfusion imaging in Pusher Syndrome to investigate the neural substrates involved in controlling upright body position. PLoS ONE 4(5): e5737

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tranel D, Bechara A, Denburg NL (2002) Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional processing. Cortex 38(4): 589–612

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turkstra L, Dixon T, Baker K (2004) Theory of Mind and social beliefs in adolescents with traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation 19: 245–256

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varley R, Siegal M (2000) Evidence for cognition without grammar from causal reasoning and ‚theory of mind‘ in an agrammatic aphasic patient. Curr Biol 10: 723–726

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Varley R, Siegal M, Want S (2001) Severe impairment in grammar does not preclude theory of mind. Neurocase 7(6): 489–493

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley K, Bussfeld P, Newen A et al (2001) Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage 14: 170-181

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley K, Fink GR (2003) Neural correlates of the first-person perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7(1): 38–42

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley K, May M, Ritzl A et al (2004) Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. J Cogn Neurosci 16(5): 817–827

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF et al (2010) Theory of mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What’s the story? Brain Language 113: 65–72

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H, Perner J (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13(1):103–128

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winner E, Brownell H, Happe´ F et al (1998) Distinguishing lies from jokes: theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Language 62: 89–106

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Young L, Dodell-Feder D, Saxe R (2010) What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia 48(9): 2658–26564

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Marx .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Marx, P., Wendel, C., Koch, M. (2012). Frontalhirninfarkte: Defizite der Theory of Mind und anderer Leistungen. In: Förstl, H. (eds) Theory of Mind. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-24916-7_29

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-24916-7_29

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-24915-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-24916-7

  • eBook Packages: Medicine (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics