Skip to main content

Missing Links in the Evolution of Language

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Characterizing Consciousness: From Cognition to the Clinic?

Part of the book series: Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences ((NEUROSCIENCE))

Abstract

The evolution of language is an intractable problem if it is assumed (a la Chomsky) that language emerged full-blown at some point during the ~ 6,000,000 year period that elapsed between the divergence of the hominid line from chimpanzees, our closest living ancestor. It is intractable because it fails to recognize the role played by the evolution of conversation, and its antecedent, non-verbal conversation, a uniquely human phenomenon. For that we have to go back to the time when bipedalism replaced knuckle walking. Bipedalism resulted in a reduction in the size of the pelvis and the consequent reduction in the size of the birth canal, a birth canal that could not accommodate an infant with a brain larger than ~1,000 cc. As a result, human infants were born with a brain and skeletal system that were much less developed than those of non-human primates. To survive, the human infant required support from her mother for a period of time that was considerably longer than that needed by the offspring of other non-human primates. As a result, the human infant spent lots of face-to-face contact time with her mother, sharing eye gaze, another uniquely human activity. I hypothesize that those interactions allowed the infant to perceive and consciously compare the contents of her and her mother’s mind. From there it is a relatively small step to joint attention, yet another uniquely human activity, which provided a basis for the assignment of names to events and objects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Lashley (1951) made a similar argument and also suggested a hierarchical, as opposed to a linear, structure of learned responses in which particular nodes controlled the spacing of particular subordinate responses. These and related ideas have been described in detail by others and will not be elaborated in this chapter (Anderson and Bower 1974). [Gardner]

References

  • Anderson JR, Bower GH (1974) Human Associative Memory. Washington, D.C., Hemisphere Publishing

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates E, Camaioni L, Volterra V (1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merill-Palmer Q 21:205–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Belzung C, Le Pape G (1994) Comparison of different behavioral test situations used in psychopharmacology for measurement of anxiety. Physiol Behav 56:623–628

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beran M, Evans T, Harris E (2008) Perception of food amount by chimpanzees based on the number, size, contour length and visibility of items. Animal Behavior 75:1793–1802

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biro D, Matsuzawa T (1999) Numerical ordering in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): planning, executing and monitoring. J Comp Psychol 113:178–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom LM, Rocissano L, Hood L (1976) Adult–child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cogn Psychol 8:521–552

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boysen ST (1993) Counting in chimpanzees: nonhuman principles and emergent properties of number. In: Boysen ST, Capaldi EJ (eds) The development of numerical competence animal and human models. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 39–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine MDS (1976) Children’s first word combinations. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 41:1–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon E, Terrace HS (1998) Ordering of the numerosities 1–9 by monkeys. Science 282:746–749

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon E, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities 1–9 by rhesus monkeys. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 25:31–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton I, Bates E (1979) The emergence of intentional communication. New Dir Child Dev 4:81–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breukelaar JW, Dalrymple-Alford JC (1998) Timing ability and numerical competence in rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 24:84–97

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown R (1970) The first sentences of child and chimpanzee. In: Brown R (ed) Psycholinguistics. Free Press, New York, pp 210–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown R (1973) A first language: the early stage. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantlon J, Brannon E (2006) Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychol Sci 17:401–406

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen S, Swartz KB, Terrace HS (1991) Preliminary evidence for the development of learning set for 4-item lists by rhesus monkeys. Paper presented at annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen S, Swartz KB, Terrace HS (1997) Knowledge of the ordinal position of list items Tein rhesus monkeys. Psychol Sci 8:80–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1959) A review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language 35:26–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1986) Knowledge of language. Praeger, Westport

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook RG, Katz JS, Cavoto BR (1997) Pigeon same–different concept learning with multiple stimulus classes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 23:417–433

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In: Barkow C, Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato MR, Colombo M (1988) Representation of serial order in monkeys (Cebus apella). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 14:131–139

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1859) On the origin of species. Mercury, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery NJ (2000) The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function, and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci Behav Rev 24:581–604

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot J, Wasserman EA, Young ME (2001) Discriminating the relation between relations: the role of entropy in abstract conceptualization by baboons (Papio papio) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 27:316–328

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1983) Modularity of mind. MIT, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Fouts RS (1975) Capacity for language in great apes. In: Tuttle R (ed) Socioecology and psychology of primates. Mouton, Chicago, pp 371–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner BT (1981) Project Nim: Who taught whom? Contemporary Psychology 26:425–426

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner H (1985) The mind’s new science: a history of the cognitive revolution. Basic, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner BT, Gardner RA (1969) Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 162:664–672

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner BT, Gardner RA (1973) Teaching sign language to the chimpanzee: Washoe. The Psychological Cinema Register, University Park

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon J (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychol Rev 84(3):279–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ (1977) Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ, Lewontin RC (1979) The spandrels of St. Marco and the Panglossian program: a critique of the adaptationist program. Proc Roy Soc London 205:281–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ, Vrba ES (1982) Exaptation: a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8:4–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, MacNeilage P, Ware M (1996) Numerical representations in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:1514–1517

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes C (1951) The ape in our house. Harper, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman LM, Richards DG, Wolz JP (1984) Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition 16:129–219

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein RJ, Loveland DH, Cable C (1976) Natural concepts in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2:285–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson P (2002) The cradle of thought: exploring the origins of thinking. Macmillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull CL (1943) Principles of behavior. Appleton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (2002) Foundations of language: brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg LA, Kellogg WN (1933) The ape and the child: a study of environmental influence and its behavior. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendrick DF, Rilling M, Stonebraker TB (1981) Stimulus control of delayed matching in pigeons: directed forgetting. Exp Anal Behav 36:241–251

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kenneally C (2007) The first word: the search for the origins for language. Viking, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornell N, Son LK, Terrace HS (2007) Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychol Sci 18:4–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladyna-Kots N (1935) Infant chimpanzee and human child. Museum Darwinianum, Moscow

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley KS (1951) The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Jeffries LA (ed) Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. Wiley, New York, pp 112–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman P (1968) Primate vocalizations and human linguistic ability. J Acoust Soc Am 44:1574–1584

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman P (1975) On the origins of language: an introduction to the evolution of human speech. Macmillan, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters JC, Rimm DC (1987) Behavior therapy: techniques and empirical findings. Harcourt, San Diego

    Google Scholar 

  • Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320–334

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt D, MacLean EL, Jaffe S, Brannon EM (2007) A comparative analysis of serial-ordering in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Comp Psychol 121:363–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles HL (1983) Apes and language: the search for communicative competence. In: de Luse J, Wilder HT (eds) Language in primates: perspectives and implications. Springer, New York, pp 43–61

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson K (1981) Individual differences in language development: implications for development and language. Dev Psychol 17:170–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson FG (1978) The gestures of a gorilla: language acquisition by another pongid. Brain Lang 12:72–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson FG (1981) Ape Language. Science 211:86–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg I (1991) Language acquisition and form in a bilingual environment: a framework for studying birdsong in zones of sympatry. Ethology 89:1–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1994) The language instinct. Morrow, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (2007) The language instinct: how the mind creates language. Harper Perennial Modern Classics, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli DJ, Vonk J (2003) Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? Trends Cogn Sci 7:157–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack D (1971) Language in a chimpanzee? Science 172:808–822

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Premack D (1976) Intelligence in Ape and Man. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack D (1986) Pedagogy and aesthetics as sources of culture. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) Handbook of cognitive neuroscience. Plenum, New York, pp 15–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:515–526

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romanes G (1884) Mental evolution in animals. AMS, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rumbaugh DM, Gill TV, von Glasersfeld EC (1973) Reading and sentence completion by a chimpanzee. Science 182:731–733

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sands SF, Wright AA (1980) Primate memory: retention of serial list items by a rhesus monkey. Science 209:938–940

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh ES (1994) Kanzi: the ape at the brink of the human mind. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Scaife M, Bruner J (1975) The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature 253:265–266

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder B (1977) Koko: the talking gorilla. Home Vision

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Shields WE, Smith JD, Washburn DA (1997) Uncertain responses by humans and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in a psychophysical same–different task. J Exp Psychol Gen 126:147–164

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner BF (1935) Two types of conditioned reflexes and a pseudo-type. J Genet Psychol 12:66–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner BF (1938) The behavior of organisms. Appleton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner BF (1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner BF (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner BF (1959) Teaching machines. Cumulative record. Appleton, New York, pp 158–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke ES, Katz G, Purcell SE, Ehrlich SM, Breinlinger K (1994) Early knowledge of object motion: continuity and inertia. Cognition 51:131–176

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer H (1886) The principles of psychology. Appleton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Spetch ML, Kelly DM (2006) Comparative spatial cognition: processes in landmark and surface-based place finding. In: Wasserman E, Zentall T (eds) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 210–228

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiegel R (2003) Psychopharmacology: an introduction. Wiley, Chichester

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (1977) The first relationship: infant and mother. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Swartz KB, Chen S, Terrace HS (1991) Serial learning by rhesus monkeys. I: Acquisition and retention of multiple four-item lists. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 17:396–410

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS (1979) Nim. Knopf, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS (1981) Reply to Bindra and Patterson. Science 211:87–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS (1982) Why Koko can’t talk. Sciences 22:8–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS (1984) Simultaneous chaining: the problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In: Commons ML, Herrnstein RJ, Wagner AR (eds) Quantitative analyses of behavior: discrimination processes. Ballinger, Cambridge, pp 115–138

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS, Petitto LA, Sanders RJ, Bever TG (1979) Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206:891–902

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS, Son L, Brannon E (2003) Serial expertise of rhesus macaques. Psychol Sci 14:66–73

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson CR, Church RM (1980) An explanation of the language of a chimpanzee. Science 208:313–314

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Bellagamba F (1999) Re-enacting intended acts: comparing 12- and 18-month-olds. Infant Behav Dev 22(2):277–282

    Google Scholar 

  • Treichler FR, Raghanti M, Van Tilburg DN (2003) Linking of serially ordered lists by macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta): list position influences. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 29:211–221

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1977) Descriptive analysis of infant communicative behavior. In: Schaffer HR (ed) Studies in mother–infant interaction. Academic, London, pp 227–270

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman EA, Bhatt A (1992) Conceptualization of natural and artificial stimuli by pigeons. In: Honig WK, Fetterman JG (eds) Cognitive aspects of stimulus control. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 203–223

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (2006) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright AA, Santiago HC, Sands SF (1984) Pigeon and monkey serial probe recognition: acquisition, strategies, and serial position effects. Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Young ME, Wasserman EA (1997) Entropy detection by pigeons: Response to mixed visual displays after same-different discrimination training. J Exp Psychol 23(2):157–170

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to H. S. Terrace .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Terrace, H.S. (2011). Missing Links in the Evolution of Language. In: Dehaene, S., Christen, Y. (eds) Characterizing Consciousness: From Cognition to the Clinic?. Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-18015-6_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics