Skip to main content

Outcome Evaluation in Decision Making: ERP Studies

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Culture and Neural Frames of Cognition and Communication

Part of the book series: On Thinking ((ONTHINKING))

Abstract

Decision making can be regarded as an outcome of mental processes (cognitive process) leading to the selection of a course of action among several alternatives. Every decision-making process produces a final choice. In this chapter, we would use the event-related brain potential (ERP) technique to study neural correlates of outcome evaluation in decision making.

Firstly, we used the motivation to be deceptive as a way to manipulate the motivational state of participants. Results showed that in both groups, losing money evoked a negative component (FRN). The amplitude of FRN was higher, and the latency shorter, in the deception group than in the simple response group. In addition, monetary losses also elicited a P3 peaking at around 400 ms, which was larger in the deception group than in the simple response group. Source modeling suggested a dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) source for the FRN and a rostral ACC source for the P300. These results suggest that motivation can influence the evaluation of performance outcomes. The FRN may reflect a process signaling that the outcome is worse than what participants expect, and P300 may be related to the emotional processing of disappointment and regret.

Secondly, in a simulated deception situation with graded monetary incentives, participants made a decision to lie or be truthful in each trial and held their response until a delayed imperative signal was presented. Spatiotemporal principal component analysis (PCA) and source analysis revealed that brain activities dominant in the left lateral frontal area approximately 800-1000 ms post-stimulus and over the central-frontal-parietal and right frontal areas after 1300 ms were significantly more negative in the deceptive condition than in the truthful condition. These results suggest that two serial cognitive processes, decision-making and response preparation, are related to deliberate deception.

Thirdly, an ERP experiment was conducted to investigate the component processes underlying error related feedback and regret following incorrect decisions. Results showed that the amplitude of the FRN following an incorrect choice, the personal responsibility people accepted for an incorrect choice, the regret they expressed about it, and the counterfactual thinking they reported were higher when they alone made the choice than when they were not alone in their incorrect choice. No differences were found on the FRN or behavioral measures when participants were joined by one versus two others in their incorrect choice. The P300 amplitude, in contrast, was inversely related to the responsibility levels, consistent with the notion that personal accountability of a choice influences the allocation of attentional and cognitive resources to the task. Together, these results are consistent with neural modeling indicating that regret promotes error related processing and learning.

Lastly, Compared with non-anxious people, anxious people are suggested to judge the negative events as more likely to happen, and more likely to interpret the ambiguous outcomes as negative ones. We predicted that the FRN should be different between high trait-anxiety (HTA) and low trait-anxiety (LTA) groups. We discovered that the amplitude of the FRN indicating negative versus positive outcomes was significantly larger for LTA participants than HTA participants. However, the intergroup difference of the FRN indicating ambiguous versus positive outcomes didn’t reach significance. The result indicated that there was a relationship between the FRN and the participants’ individual differences in anxiety, which possibly reflected the impact of anxiety on outcome expectation. This finding provided insight about the underlying features of anxiety.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ambach W, Stark R, Peper M, Vaitl D (2008) An interfering go/no-go task does not affect accuracy in a concealed information test. Int J Psychophysiol 68(1):6–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armoundas AA, Feldman AB, Sherman DA, Cohen RJ (2001) Applicability of the single equivalent point dipole model to represent a spatially distributed bio-electrical source. Med Biol Eng Comput 39(5):562–570

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balconi M, Crivelli D (2010) FRN and P300 ERP effect modulation in response to feedback sensitivity: the contribution of punishment-reward system (BIS/BAS) and behaviour identification of action. Neurosci Res 66(2):162–172

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker G, Hagemann D, Bartussek D, Naumann E, Schneider C (2004) Stimulus analysis and response organization in the CNV-paradigm: ERP studies about extraversion, cognitive information processing, and motor preparation. Pers Individ Dif 36(4):893–911

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell DE (1982) Regret in decision-making under uncertainty. Oper Res 30(5):961–981

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellebaum C, Daum I (2008) Learning-related changes in reward expectancy are reflected in the feedback-related negativity. Eur J Neurosci 27(7):1823–1835

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernat E, Bunce S, Shevrin H (2001) Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. Int J Psychophysiol 42:11–34

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair K, Marsh AA, Morton J, Vythilingam M, Jones M, Mondillo K et al (2006) Choosing the lesser of two evils, the better of two goods: specifying the roles of ventromedial prefrontal cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate in object choice. J Neurosci 26(44):11379–11386

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunia CHM, van Boxtel GJM (2001) Wait and see. Int J Psychophysiol 43(1):59–75

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 4:215–222

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Camille N, Coricelli G, Sallet J, Pradat-Diehl P, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A (2004) The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science 304:1167–1170

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell KB, Courchesne E, Picton TW, Squires KC (1979) Evoked-potential correlates of human information-processing. Biol Psychol 8:45–68

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Canterbury R, Golden AM, Taghavi R, Neshat-Doost H, Moradi A, Yule W (2004) Anxiety and judgements about emotional events in children and adolescents. Pers Individ Dif 36(3):695–704

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CarriĂłn R, Keenan J, Sebanz N (2009) A truth that’s told with bad intent: an ERP study of deception. Cognition 114(1):105–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu P, Ambady N, Deldin P (2004) Contingent negative variation to emotional in-and out-group stimuli differentiates high-and low-prejudiced individuals. J Cogn Neurosci 16(10):1830–1839

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MD (2008) Economics. Learning with regret. Science 319(5866):1052–1053

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MX, Ranganath C (2007) Reinforcement learning signals predict future decisions. J Neurosci 27:371–378

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MX, Elger CE, Ranganath C (2007) Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. Neuroimage 35(2):968–978

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly T, Zeelenberg M (2002) Regret in decision making. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11(6):212–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli G, Critchley HD, Joffily M, O’Doherty JP, Sirigu A, Dolan RJ (2005) Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nat Neurosci 8:1255–1262

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli G, Dolan RJ, Sirigu A (2007) Brain, emotion and decision making: the paradigmatic example of regret. Trends Cogn Sci 11(6):258–265

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damen E, Brunia C (1994) Is a stimulus conveying task-relevant information a sufficient condition to elicit a stimulus-preceding negativity? Psychophysiology 31:129

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson ME (1980) Physiological detection of deception: measurement of responses to questions and answers during countermeasure maneuvers. Psychophysiology 17(1):8–17

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo BM, Kashy DA, Kirkendol SE, Wyer MM, Epstein JA (1996) Lying in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol 70(5):979–995

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo BM, Lindsay JJ, Malone BE, Muhlenbruck L, Charlton K, Cooper H (2003) Cues to deception. Psychol Bull 129(1):74–118

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donchin E, Coles MGH (1988) Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating. Behav Brain Sci 11:357–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donkers FCL, Nieuwenhuis S, van Boxtel GJM (2005) Mediofrontal negativities in the absence of responding. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 25:777–787

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan-Johnson C, Donchin E (1977) On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. Psychophysiology 14(5):456–467

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg AE, Baron J, Seligman MEP (1998) Individual difference in risk aversion and anxiety. Psychol Bull 87:245–251

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott R, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ (2000) Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. J Neurosci 20:6159–6165

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstude K, Roese NJ (2007) Beyond rationality: counterfactual thinking and behavior regulation. Behav Brain Sci 30(5–6):457

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabiani M, Gratton G, Federmeier KD (2007) Event-related potentials: methods, theory, and applications. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG (eds) Handbook of psychophysiology, 3rd edn. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 85–119

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1991) Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78:447–455

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fang F, Liu Y, Shen Z (2003) Lie detection with contingent negative variation. Int J Psychophysiol 50(3):247–255

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Furedy JJ, Ben-Shakhar G (1991) The roles of deception, intention to deceive, and motivation to avoid detection in the psychophysiological detection of guilty knowledge. Psychophysiology 28(2):163–171

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Furedy JJ, Davis C, Gurevich M (1988) Differentiation of deception as a psychological process – a psychophysiological approach. Psychophysiology 25:683–688

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky A, Moskowitz G (2000) Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alternatives. J Exp Soc Psychol 36(4):384–409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR (2002) The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 295:2279–2282

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR (2004) Are all medial frontal negativities created equal? Toward a richer empirical basis for theories of action monitoring. In: Ullsperger M, Falkenstein M (eds) Conflicts, and the brain: current opinions on performance monitoring. Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Leipzig, pp 14–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993) A neural system for error-detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4:385–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein RZ, Cottone LA, Jia Z, Maloney T, Volkow ND, Squires NK (2006) The effect of graded monetary reward on cognitive event-related potentials and behavior in young healthy adults. Int J Psychophysiol 62(2):272–279

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goethals GR, Cooper J (1975) When dissonance is reduces. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2):361–367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gu RL, Huang YX, Luo YJCA (2010) Anxiety and feedback negativity. Psychophysiology 47:289–298

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, McDonald N, Simons RF (2003) Anxiety and error-related brain activity. Biol Psychol 64(1–2):77–90

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, Holroyd CB, Moser JS, Simons RF (2005) Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. Psychophysiology 42:161–170

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, Moser JS, Holroyd CB, Simons RF (2006) The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes. Biol Psychol 71:148–154

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, Moser JS, Holroyd CB, Simons RF (2007) It’s worse than you thought: the feedback negativity and violations of reward prediction in gambling tasks. Psychophysiology 44(6):905–912

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hampton AN, Bossaerts P, O’Doherty JP (2006) The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in abstract state-based inference during decision making in humans. J Neurosci 26(32):8360–8367

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hewig J, Trippe R, Hecht H, Coles MGH, Holroyd CB, Miltner WHR (2007) Decision-making in blackjack: an electrophysiological analysis. Cereb Cortex 17:865–877

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Coles MGH (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109:679–709

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Coles MGH (2008) Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex integrates reinforcement history to guide voluntary behaviour. Cortex 44(5):548–559

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Cohen JD (2003) Errors in reward prediction are reflected in the event-related brain potential. Neuroreport 14(18):2481–2484

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Larsen JT, Cohen JD (2004a) Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. Psychophysiology 41:245–253

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Nystrom L, Mars RB, Coles MGH, Cohen JD (2004b) Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals. Nat Neurosci 7:497–498

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Yeung N, Coles M, Cohen J (2005) A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. J Exp Psychol Gen 134(2):163–191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Hajcak G, Larsen JT (2006) The good, the bad and the neutral: electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli. Brain Res 1105(1):93–101

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Itagaki S, Katayama J (2008) Self-relevant criteria determine the evaluation of outcomes induced by others. Neuroreport 19(3):383–387

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito TA, Larsen JT, Smith NK, Cacioppo JT (1998) Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. J Pers Soc Psychol 75:887–900

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson JR (2006) Toward a neurocognitive basis of deception. J Credibility Assess Witness Psychol 7(2):41–46

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson R, Donchin E (1978) On how P300 amplitude varies with utility of eliciting stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 44:424–437

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson R, Donchin E (1985) Second thoughts – multiple P300s elicited by a single stimulus. Psychophysiology 22:182–194

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson MM, Rosenfeld JP (1992) Oddball-evoked P300-based method of deception detection in the laboratory II: utilization of nonselective activation of relevant knowledge. Int J Psychophysiol 12:289–306

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson JR, Barnhardt J, Zhu J (2003) The deceptive response: effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity. Biol Psychol 64(3):217–253

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson JR, Barnhardt J, Zhu J (2004) The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding. Neuropsychologia 42(7):878–901

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson R, Barnhardt J, Zhu J (2005) Differential effects of practice on the executive processes used for truthful and deceptive responses: an event-related brain potential study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24:386–404

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson JR, Henkell H, Simon E, Zhu J (2008) The self in conflict: the role of executive processes during truthful and deceptive responses about attitudes. Neuroimage 39(1):469–482

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Tversky A (2000) Choice, values, frames. Cambridge University Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Latane B, Darley JM (1968) Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(3):215–221

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lauriola M, Levin IP (2001) Personality traits and risky decision-making in a controlled experimental task: an exploratory study. Pers Individ Dif 31(2):215–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt SD, List JA (2008) Homo economicus Evolves, Science, 319(5865):909–910

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leynes P, Allen J, Marsh R (1998) Topographic differences in CNV amplitude reflect different preparatory processes. Int J Psychophysiol 31(1):33–44

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loomes G, Sugden R (1982) Regret theory: an alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. Econ J 92(368):805–824

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luu P, Tucker DM, Derryberry D, Reed M, Poulsen C (2003) Electrophysiological responses to errors and feedback in the process of action regulation. Psychol Sci 14:47–53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maier M, Steinhauser M, HĂĽbner R (2008) Is the error-related negativity amplitude related to error detectability? Evidence from effects of different error types. J Cogn Neurosci 20:2263–2273

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maner JK, Richey JA, Cromer K, Mallott M, Lejuez CW, Joiner TE et al (2007) Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. Pers Individ Differ 42(4):665–675

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchiori D, Warglien M (2008) Predicting human interactive learning by regret-driven neural networks. Science 319(5866):1111–1113

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers BA, McGraw AP (2001) Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 10(6):210–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers BA, Schwartz A, Ho K, Ritov I (1997) Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychol Sci 8(6):423–429

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers B, Schwartz A, Ritov I (1999) Emotion-based choice. J Exp Psychol Gen 128(3):332–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miltner WHR, Braun CH, Coles MGH (1997) Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J Cogn Neurosci 9:788–798

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka S, Watson D, Clark LA (1998) Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annu Rev Psychol 49:377–412

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitte K (2007) Anxiety and risk decision-making: the role of subjective probability and subjective cost of negative events. Pers Individ Differ 43(2):243–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moser JS, Simons RF (2009) The neural consequences of flip-flopping: the feedback-related negativity and salience of reward prediction. Psychophysiology 46(2):313–320

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muris P, van der Heiden S (2006) Anxiety, depression, and judgments about the probability of future negative and positive events in children. J Anxiety Disord 20(2):252–261

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuis S, Holroyd CB, Mol N, Coles MGH (2004a) Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and functional significance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28(4):441–448

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Holroyd CB, Schurger A, Cohen JD (2004b) Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. Cereb Cortex 14:741–747

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C (2001) Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:95–102

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Picton TW, Hillyard SA, Galambos R (1976) Habituation and attention in the auditory system. In: Keidel WD, Neff WD (eds) Handbook of sensory physiology, vol. V/3. Auditory system, clinical and special topics. Springer, Berlin, pp 343–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118(10):2128–2148

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reason J (1990) Human error. Cambridge University Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Reb J (2008) Regret aversion and decision process quality: effects of regret salience on decision process carefulness. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 105(2):169–182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld JP, Reinhart AM, Bhatt M, Ellwanger J, Gora K, Sekera M, Sweet J (1998) P300 correlates of simulated malingered amnesia in a matching-to-sample task: topographic analyses of deception versus truthtelling responses. Int J Psychophysiol 28:233–247

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruchsow M, Grothe J, Spitzer M, Kiefer M (2002) Human anterior cingulate cortex is activated by negative feedback: evidence from event-related potentials in a guessing task. Neurosci Lett 325:203–206

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rushworth MFS, Behrens TEJ (2008) Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 11(4):389–397

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sato A, Yasuda A, Ohira H, Miyawaki K, Nishikawa M, Kumano H, Kuboki TL (2005) Effects of value and reward magnitude on feedback negativity and P300. Neuroreport 16:407–411

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shek DT (1993) The Chinese version of the state-trait anxiety inventory: its relationship to different measures of psychological well-being. J Clin Psychol 49(3):349–358

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA (1983) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologist Press, Palo Alto

    Google Scholar 

  • Squires KC, Hillyard SA, Lindsay PH (1973) Cortical potentials evoked by confirming and disconfirming deedback following an auditory discrimination. Percept Psychophys 13:25–31

    Google Scholar 

  • Sun SY, Mai X, Liu C, Liu JY, Luo YJ. The processes leading to deception: ERP spatiotemporal principal component analysis and source analysis. J Soc Neurosci, in press

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER (1965) Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 150(3700):1187–1188

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler RH, Sunstein CR (2008) Economic policy for humans, The Boston Globe, 17 April 2008

    Google Scholar 

  • Toyomaki A, Murohashi H (2005) The ERPs to feedback indicating monetary loss and gain on the game of modified “rock–paper–scissors”. Int Congr Ser 1278:381–384

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY (2003) Error monitoring using external feedback: specific roles of the habenular complex, the reward system, and the cingulate motor area revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 23:4308–4314

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Boxtel G, Becker K (2004) Cortical measures of anticipation. J Psychophysiol 18(2–3):61–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrij A, Mann S (2001) Telling and detecting lies in a high-stake situation: the case of a convicted murderer. Appl Cogn Psychol 15(2):187–203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrij A, Fisher R, Mann S, Leal S (2006) Detecting deception by manipulating cognitive load. Trends Cogn Sci 10(4):141–142

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Y, Yang F (1995) Dynamic extraction of visual evoked potentials through spatial analysis and dipole localization. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 42(8):762–768

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weerts T, Lang P (1973) The effects of eye fixation and stimulus and response location on the contingent negative variation (CNV). Biol Psychol 1(1):1–19

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wrase J, Kahnt T, Schiagenhauf F, Beck A, Cohen MX, Knutson B et al (2007) Different neural systems adjust motor behavior in response to reward and punishment. Neuroimage 36(4):1253–1262

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yager RR (2004) Decision making using minimization of regret. Int J Approximate Reason 36(2):109–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yasuda A, Sato A, Miyawaki K, Kumano H, Kuboki T (2004) Error-related negativity reflects detection of negative reward prediction error. Neuroreport 15(16):2561–2565

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung N (2004) Relating cognitive and affective theories of the error-related negativity. In: Ullsperger M, Falkenstein M (eds) Errors, conflicts, and the brain: current opinions on performance monitoring. Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Leipzig, pp 63–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung N, Sanfey AG (2004) Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J Neurosci 24:6258–6264

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung N, Holroyd CB, Cohen JD (2005) ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. Cereb Cortex 15:535–544

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M (1999) Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision making. J Behav Decis Mak 12(2):93–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, Beattie J (1997) Consequences of regret aversion 2: additional evidence for effects of feedback on decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 72(1):63–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, Beattie J, van der Pligt J, de Vries NK (1996) Consequences of regret aversion: effects of expected feedback on risky decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 65(2):148–158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, van Dijk WW, Manstead ASR (1998) Reconsidering the relation between regret and responsibility. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 74(3):254–272

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, van Dijk WW, Manstead AS (2000) Regret and responsibility resolved? Evaluating Ordóñez and Connolly’s (2000) conclusions. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 81(1):143–154

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang HJ, Zhou LM, Luo YJ (2009) The influence of responsbility on the regret intensity: an ERP study. Acta Psychol Sin 41(5):454–463

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman M, DePaulo BM, Rosenthal R (1981) Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. In: Berkowitz L (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology, vol 14. Acadamic, New York, pp 1–59

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yue-Jia Luo .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Luo, YJ., Sun, SY., Mai, XQ., Gu, RL., Zhang, HJ. (2011). Outcome Evaluation in Decision Making: ERP Studies. In: Han, S., Pöppel, E. (eds) Culture and Neural Frames of Cognition and Communication. On Thinking. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-15423-2_16

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics