Advertisement

Implicit Spatial Length Modulates Time Estimates, But Not Vice Versa

  • Roberto Bottini
  • Daniel Casasanto
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 6222)

Abstract

How are space and time represented in the human mind? Here we evaluate two theoretical proposals, one suggesting a symmetric relationship between space and time (ATOM theory) and the other an asymmetric relationship (metaphor theory). In Experiment 1, Dutch-speakers saw 7-letter nouns that named concrete objects of various spatial lengths (tr. pencil, bench, footpath) and estimated how much time they remained on the screen. In Experiment 2, participants saw nouns naming temporal events of various durations (tr. blink, party, season) and estimated the words’ spatial length. Nouns that named short objects were judged to remain on the screen for a shorter time, and nouns that named longer objects to remain for a longer time. By contrast, variations in the duration of the event nouns’ referents had no effect on judgments of the words’ spatial length. This asymmetric pattern of cross-dimensional interference supports metaphor theory and challenges ATOM.

Keywords

ATOM Metaphor Psychophysics Space Time 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Walsh, V.: A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cog. Sci. 7(11), 483–488 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Basso, G., Nichelli, P., Frassinetti, F., di Pellegrino, G.: Time perception in a neglected space. Neuroreport 7, 2111–2114 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Church, R.M., Meck, W.H.: The numerical attribute of stimuli. In: Roitblat, H.L., Beaver, T.G., Terrace, H.S. (eds.) Animal Cognition, pp. 445–464. Erlbaum, Mahwah (1984)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Fischer, M.: Cognitive representation of negative numbers. Psych. Sci. 14(3), 278–282 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Gallistell, R.C., Gellman, R.: Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends in Cog. Sci. 4, 59–65 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E.D., Cipolotti, L.: Dissociations and interactions between time, numerosity and space processing. Neuropsychologia 47(13), 2732–2748 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Lakoff, G., Johnson, M.: Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1999)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Clark, H.H.: Space, time, semantics and the child. In: Moore, T.E. (ed.) Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language, pp. 27–63. Academic Press, New York (1973)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Boroditsky, L.: Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition 75(1), 1–28 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Casasanto, D., Fotakopoulou, O., Boroditsky, L.: Space and Time in the Child’s Mind: Evidence for a Cross-Dimensional Asymmetry. Cog. Sci. 34, 387–405 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Casasanto, D., Boroditsky, L.: Time in the Mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition 106, 579–593 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Santiago, J., Román, A., Ouellet, M.: Flexible foundations of abstract thought: A review and a theory (submitted)Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Pansky, A., Algom, D.: Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgement of numerals: The role of attention. JEP:HPP 25, 39–58 (1999)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Garner, W.R.: Interaction of Stimulus Dimensions in Concept and Choice Processes. Cog. Psych. 8, 98–123 (1976)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Ornstein, R.: On the experience of time. Penguin, Hammondsworth (1969)Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Miles, L.K., Nind, L.K., Macrae, C.N.: Moving Through Time. Psych. Sci. (2010), doi:10.1177/0956797609359333Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Roberto Bottini
    • 1
    • 2
  • Daniel Casasanto
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Human SciencesUniversity of BergamoBergamo
  2. 2.Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNeurobiology of Language GroupNijmegen
  3. 3.Donders Center for Brain, Cognition, and BehaviorRadboud UniversityNijmegen

Personalised recommendations